Predicting transfer of training through the unified model of task-specific motivation

Joost Jansen in de Wal (Research Institute of Child Development and Education, Universiteit van Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
Bas de Jong (Research Institute of Child Development and Education, Universiteit van Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
Frank Cornelissen (Research Institute of Child Development and Education, Universiteit van Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands)
Cornelis de Brabander (Faculty of Social and Behavioural Sciences, Instituut Pedagogische Wetenschappen Onderwijswetenschappen, Leiden University, Leiden, The Netherlands)

The Learning Organization

ISSN: 0969-6474

Article publication date: 11 July 2023

Issue publication date: 30 November 2023

1120

Abstract

Purpose

This study aims to investigate the merits of the unified model of task-specific motivation (UMTM) in predicting transfer of training and to investigate (relationships between) changes in UMTM components over time. In doing so, this study takes the multidimensionality of transfer motivation into account.

Design/methodology/approach

The authors collected data among 514 employees of the judiciary who filled in the UMTM questionnaire directly after the training and after three weeks. The data were analyzed by means of structural equation modelling.

Findings

The outcomes show that transfer motivation predicts transfer intention and transfer of training over time. Moreover, the study shows that (change in) transfer motivation is predicted by (change in) personal and contextual factors identified by the UMTM as antecedents of motivation.

Originality/value

This study describes the first longitudinal evaluation of the UMTM in the literature and shows its applicability for predicting transfer of training. It is also one of the few studies that investigate transfer motivation multidimensionally and the role it plays for transfer of training. As such, this study informs other transfer of training models about the nature of transfer motivation and how transfer of training could be predicted.

Keywords

Citation

Jansen in de Wal, J., de Jong, B., Cornelissen, F. and Brabander, C.d. (2023), "Predicting transfer of training through the unified model of task-specific motivation", The Learning Organization, Vol. 30 No. 6, pp. 834-856. https://doi.org/10.1108/TLO-09-2022-0112

Publisher

:

Emerald Publishing Limited

Copyright © 2023, Joost Jansen in de Wal, Bas de Jong, Frank Cornelissen and Cornelis de Brabander.

License

Published by Emerald Publishing Limited. This article is published under the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY 4.0) licence. Anyone may reproduce, distribute, translate and create derivative works of this article (for both commercial & non-commercial purposes), subject to full attribution to the original publication and authors. The full terms of this licence may be seen at http://creativecommons.org/licences/by/4.0/legalcode


Introduction

Transfer of training, the application in the work environment of knowledge and skills that employees gained during training, is essential for employees and organizations to keep up with – and respond to – innovations and the fast-paced changes in today’s work (Grossman & Salas, 2011; Poell, 2017; Segers & Gegenfurtner, 2013). However, despite the large sums of money that organizations invest in training every year (Beer et al., 2016), transfer of training is limited (Botke et al., 2018).

Over the past decades, research has shown that transfer motivation is a pivotal factor in determining transfer of training. Moreover, transfer motivation plays an essential role in explaining why other factors, like characteristics of employees, their work contexts and training designs, influence transfer of training (see Grossman & Salas, 2011; Jacot, Raemdonck & Frenay, 2015; Tonhäuser & Büker, 2016 for reviews).

Nevertheless, transfer motivation is often not conceptualized in accordance with current theories of motivation (Gegenfurtner, 2013; Gegenfurtner et al., 2009). Many studies approach transfer motivation as a one-dimensional construct. However, current motivational theories stress that motivation has multiple dimensions that uniquely determine behaviour (Deci & Ryan, 2000; Harackiewicz et al., 2002). Research has shown that qualitatively different types of motivation for transfer of training relate differently to transfer intentions and/or transfer of training (Gegenfurtner, 2013; Gegenfurtner et al., 2016). As such, much research to date on the effects and explanatory role of transfer motivation can be refined through using contemporary motivational theory (Jacot et al., 2015). Conceptualizing transfer motivation in accordance with current motivational theory creates a challenge, however, since there are many different theories of motivation (see Eccles & Wigfield, 2002 for a review).

To overcome this challenge, recent efforts have been made to combine multiple theories into a unified model of task-specific motivation (UMTM; De Brabander & Martens, 2014). In short, the UMTM does so by acknowledging that these theories all include cognitive and affective motivational components that predict motivated behaviour. The model also identifies personal and contextual antecedents that can be translated into factors which determine these motivational components and explains why they do so.

Researchers have started to test the UMTM in search of empirical evidence for its assumptions (De Brabander & Glastra, 2018a; de Brabander & Martens, 2018b; De Brabander & Glastra, 2021). These studies showed that the collected data could be modelled adequately through the dynamics of the UMTM. However, the merits of the UMTM are not yet fully understood. Thus far, it is not yet tested whether the dynamics of the model also translate to the transfer of training context. Moreover, it is unclear whether the model can be used to predict actual task-specific action, including transfer of training. As such, it is unclear whether the UMTM can be used in different contexts to predict task-specific motivation and action and whether the UMTM can be of added value in explaining which personal and contextual antecedents predict transfer of training. If this is the case, the UMTM could be used by practitioners and researchers as a framework to examine predictors of transfer of training. Finally, previous studies have relied on cross-sectional data, which precludes conclusions about how components of the model change over time or how possible changes are related to each other. This can also be translated to the transfer of training literature, in which personal and contextual antecedents and transfer motivation are often examined once (Ford, Baldwin, & Prasad, 2018). It is, however, unknown how these components change over time among individuals (Ford et al., 2018; Huang et al., 2017). Yet, insight into the within-person dynamics of personal and contextual antecedents and transfer motivation can disentangle how going through transfer of training opportunities affects these components and whether this has an influence on the transfer of training. Therefore, the aim of this study is to investigate the tenability of the UMTM in predicting transfer of training and to investigate (relationships between) changes in UMTM components over time.

The unified model of task-specific motivation

The UMTM (see De Brabander & Martens, 2014 for an in-depth discussion of the UMTM) proposes motivational dynamics that predict task-specific action, of which transfer of training is an example. The UMTM integrates six theories of motivation, namely, self-determination theory (Ryan & Deci, 2020), expectancy-value theory (Eccles & Wigfield, 2020), flow theory (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990), self-efficacy theory (Schunk & DiBenedetto, 2020), the person-object theory of interest (Krapp, 2002) and the theory of planned behaviour (Ajzen & Fishbein, 2008). The model’s main argument for doing so is that these theories all include affective and cognitive motivational components that are theorized to precede motivated behaviour.

Moreover, the UMTM integrates personal and contextual antecedents that are predicted by several motivational theories to affect affective and cognitive motivational components. In addition to the overlap between motivational theories that the UMTM addresses, links between the UMTM and existing transfer of training models (Grossman & Salas, 2011; Holton, 1996; Massenberg et al., 2017) can be drawn. These existing models identify personal factors (e.g., personal feelings of autonomy and perceived utility of training) and contextual factors (e.g., social norms and supervisor support) as predictors of transfer motivation. These components are also part of the UMTM. However, in contrast to existing transfer of training models, the UMTM hypothesizes about the complex interrelationships between these personal and contextual components. As such, the UMTM can inform transfer of training models about interrelationships among factors included in them (see Figure 1 for an overview of the UMTM).

De Brabander & Martens (2014) argue that two conceptually independent regulation systems, one affective and one cognitive, give rise to two types of motivational valences that energize and direct task-specific action (cf. Krapp, 2002). Within the transfer of training context, these two valences together would be conceptualized as transfer motivation. The affective valences are defined as appraisals of feelings that are aroused by an activity. Cognitive valences are the product of the evaluation of action consequences. Cognitive valences can refer to the value of an action that individuals see for themselves or for others. As such, the UMTM distinguishes between personal and non-personal cognitive valences.

Both valences can be positive or negative. Positive valences instigate individuals to approach actions to realize them, whereas negative valences lead to avoidance behaviour (Elliot, 2006). In the context of transfer of training, seeing the gains in work efficiency that applying a learned skill would yield (for oneself or for others) is an example of a positive cognitive valence. A negative cognitive valence would be to expect a higher workload (for oneself or for others) as a consequence of applying new skills. A positive affective valence would entail feeling excited to practice a newly learned skill at work. Finally, feeling frustrated about having to apply such a skill would be an example of a negative affective valence. As displayed in Figure 1, it is theorized that all types of valences can be relatively independent and, in interaction, are combined into an overall valence appraisal. This valence appraisal determines individuals’ willingness to execute an activity (i.e., readiness for action, of which transfer intention is an example) and, subsequently, action (De Brabander & Martens, 2014). These assumptions are in line with transfer of training literature that showed that different types of transfer motivation predict transfer intention (Gegenfurtner, 2013; Gegenfurtner & Quesada-Pallarès, 2022) and that transfer intention predicts transfer of training (Gegenfurtner, 2013; Gegenfurtner & Testers, 2022). Figure 2 provides an overview of the expected relationships between the valences, readiness for action and action.

This leads to the following hypotheses:

H1.

Positive affective and cognitive valence positively predict readiness for action.

H2.

Negative affective and cognitive valences negatively predict readiness for action.

H3.

Readiness for action positively predicts action.

The affective and cognitive valence components described by the UMTM differ from types of motivation distinguished in self-determination theory and expectancy-value theories. In self-determination theory (Ryan & Deci, 2020), the affective component is intrinsic motivation and the cognitive component is distinguished in different types of extrinsic motivation. Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation are fundamentally opposed, as more extrinsic motivation diminishes intrinsic motivation. In expectancy-value theories (Ajzen & Fishbein, 2008; Eccles & Wigfield, 2020), on the other hand, affective and cognitive components are considered different facets of value that sum up to a total value. The UMTM differs from these theories by conceptualizing affective and cognitive valences as the resultant of different, conceptually independent systems of behaviour regulation (cf. Krapp, 2002), implying that both oppositional and additive relations can occur.

None of the motivational theories included in the UMTM scrutinize the distinction between approach and avoidance motivation. Ever since the inception of the expectancy-value theory of achievement motivation, costs have been a facet of subjective value (cf. Eccles & Wigfield, 2020). Costs simply add up (negatively) to subjective value. The UMTM recognizes costs as negative valences which generate avoidance motivation. As such, the UMTM identifies the run-up to and sustainment of every single activity fundamentally as an approach-avoidance conflict, also in cases where superficially no sign of conflict is apparent.

Task-specific antecedents

In addition to the notion of valences, the UMTM also integrates theories in terms of task-specific antecedents of these valences. The model distinguishes between three categories of personal and contextual antecedents in this respect. The first category refers to individuals’ appraisals of completing an action successfully. Most theories of motivation (e.g., self-efficacy theory; Schunk & DiBenedetto, 2020) include some construct that represents individuals’ sense of personal competence to predict motivation. In the UMTM, sense of personal competence refers to the extent to which individuals perceive themselves as capable of enacting task-specific behaviour (e.g., Am I able to apply the training content in practice?; De Brabander & Martens, 2014). In addition, Authors et al. (2014) argue that individuals’ appraisals of success depend on whether they perceive their context to be supportive of the action they are to perform. They would argue, for example, that it is impossible to expect someone to successfully apply a newly learned IT skill at work if the right technology to do so is not provided, regardless of one’s own sense of personal competence. This perceived external support, together with a sense of personal competence, results in a feasibility appraisal about performing a task successfully (De Brabander & Martens, 2014).

Based on self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 2000), indirect effects of feasibility appraisal on transfer intention are expected. These effects are also in line with transfer of training literature indicating that self-efficacy (i.e., sense of personal competence) is a predictor of transfer motivation (Massenberg et al., 2017). Moreover, support from the work floor (e.g., supervisory support and availability of necessary resources) has been found to be a predictor of transfer motivation (Massenberg et al., 2017; Salamon et al., 2022). Based on the theory of planned behaviour (Ajzen & Fishbein, 2008) and expectancy-value theory of achievement motivation (Wigfield & Eccles, 2000; Eccles & Wigfield, 2020), possible direct effects are anticipated as well, and such effects have been found in previous studies investigating the UMTM (De Brabander & Glastra, 2018a; de Brabander & Martens, 2018b; De Brabander & Glastra, 2021) and also in the transfer of training literature for self-efficacy (Gegenfurtner & Testers, 2022). Figure 3 provides an overview of the expected relationships between sense of personal competence, perceived external support, feasibility appraisal and the valences.

This leads to the following hypotheses:

H4.

Feasibility appraisal positively predicts positive affective and cognitive valences.

H5.

Feasibility appraisal negatively predicts negative affective and cognitive valences.

The second category of antecedents concerns individuals’ autonomy. This category, too, distinguishes between a personal and a contextual factor. The personal factor is described by individuals’ sense of personal autonomy, which is defined as the extent to which people experience themselves to be the origin of choosing and performing an action scheme (e.g., experiencing volition to decide how and when training content is being applied). The contextual factor refers to perceived freedom of action, which is defined as the extent to which individuals experience freedom to make decisions about the selection and performance of an action scheme granted by their contexts (e.g., freedom to make choices in applying training content). In line with self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 2000), perceived freedom of action is hypothesized to contribute to individuals’ sense of personal autonomy, which, in turn, affects transfer intention through valence appraisals. Between sense of personal autonomy and affective valence, a reciprocal relationship is expected (De Brabander & Martens, 2014). These assumptions corroborate transfer of training literature that indicates that autonomy in applying training content is a positive predictor of transfer motivation (Colquitt, 2000). Empirical explorations of the UMTM showed that sense of personal autonomy was also directly related with transfer intention (De Brabander & Glastra, 2018a; de Brabander & Martens, 2018b; De Brabander & Glastra, 2021; see Figure 4 for an overview of the expected relationships between perceived freedom of action, sense of personal autonomy and the valences).

This leads to the following hypotheses:

H6.

Perceived freedom of action positively predicts sense of personal autonomy.

H7.

Sense of personal autonomy positively predicts positive cognitive valences.

H8.

Sense of personal autonomy negatively predicts negative cognitive valences.

H9.

Sense of personal autonomy has a positive reciprocal association with positive affective valence.

H10.

Sense of personal autonomy has a negative reciprocal association with negative affective valence.

The final category of antecedents revolves around social factors. In the theory of planned behaviour, subjective norm is included as a social factor. It refers to the tendency to abide by the approval or disapproval of significant others regarding particular behaviours (e.g., negative reactions from colleagues when training content is being applied in practice; Ajzen & Fishbein, 2008). This tendency is theorized to depend on the extent to which people feel connected to others who participate in the context of an activity (i.e., sense of personal relatedness; Deci & Ryan, 2000). As feelings of relatedness with colleagues were not assumed to vary over instances of transferring training content, it was not considered a task-specific predictor of transfer of training. As such, relatedness was not investigated in this study. Subjective norm is hypothesized to have an indirect effect on transfer intention via cognitive and affective valences. In line with the theory of planned behaviour, subjective norm is expected to have a possible direct effect on transfer intention as well (De Brabander & Martens, 2014). Empirically this effect could be confirmed but depended on the characteristics of the activity (De Brabander & Glastra, 2018a; de Brabander & Martens, 2018b). Moreover, within the transfer of training literature, Gilpin-Jackson and Bushe (2007) found that positive organizational norms towards applying training content enhance transfer of training. However, they did not investigate the effect of organizational norms on transfer motivation. Figure 5 provides an overview of the expected relationships between subjective norm, sense of personal relatedness and the valences.

This leads to the following hypotheses:

H11.

Subjective norm positively predicts positive affective and cognitive valences.

H12.

Subjective norm negatively predicts negative affective and cognitive valences.

Studies investigating the UMTM showed that data could be modelled in accordance with the UMTM when it was used to predict teachers’ intention to perform professionalization activities. However, whether and to what extent different UMTM components were related to each other depended on the characteristics of the specific activity. Subjective norm, for instance, played a more pervasive role for activities that were more social in character (De Brabander & Glastra, 2018a; de Brabander & Martens, 2018b). Moreover, affective valences were more in balance with cognitive valences for personally decided activities, whereas for board and team decided activities, cognitive valences dominated (De Brabander & Glastra, 2018a). In all studies, however, sense of competence played a less prominent role than could be expected from motivational theory.

In the current formulation of the UMTM time of appraisal is not determined. People have appraisals of the components of the model before, during and after the performance of an activity (De Brabander & Glastra, 2018a). Time of appraisal does matter, though, for instance, because appraisals in advance will be more uncertain than during or after an activity, but appraisals of the components of the model can be made at different points in time. However, previous studies investigating the UMTM have not yet investigated how these appraisals in the components of the model change over time and what impact they have on transfer motivation, transfer intention and transfer of training. In line with the assumed cross-sectional relationships between the UMTM components, it could be expected that changes among components over time are related to the other components of the UMTM. For example, if cross-sectional results show that feasibility appraisal positively predicts positive affective valence, it is expected that changes between these constructs are also related to each other. This leads to the following hypothesis:

H13.

Changes in the UMTM components relate to each other in accordance with cross-sectional relationships.

The present study

To sum up, previous research did not investigate the extent to which the UMTM predicted actual task-specific action (of which transfer of training is an example) nor studied (relationships between) changes in UMTM components. Moreover, we expect that the transfer of training can be explained and predicted through the dynamics of the UMTM. This leads to the following research questions:

RQ1.

To what extent does the UMTM predict transfer of training among employees?

RQ2.

How do UMTM components change after going through transfer opportunities and how are these changes related to each other?

Method

Sample and procedure

Data were collected among employees of the Dutch justice system. These employees participated in training courses that were organized by the study centre for the judiciary. These courses ranged in aim from acquiring knowledge about the legislation of the European Union to learning to compose a well-written verdict. Most courses focused on providing knowledge (84.4%), whereas a minor proportion (15.6%) focused on providing skills. Courses were considered suitable for inclusion in this study if they allowed for data collection during and directly after the course, more than ten participants were scheduled to attend, and there was a substantial probability that participants would have opportunities to use the learned content in practice within three weeks after finalizing the course. One hundred and two courses were initially selected, with a total of 1,410 prospective participants for this study. The length of the courses ranged between one day and six weeks. The amount of contact days during the courses ranged between one and eight full days. All trainings were provided in-person. Course trainers asked participants to fill in digital questionnaires at two time points, once directly after the course and once three weeks later. The time between data collections was set to allow participants to transfer training content to practice. Based on these two data waves, we could examine the ability of the UMTM to predict transfer of training and investigate changes in the UMTM components over time. Participation was voluntary, and no incentives were offered. At T1, N = 514 participants from 100 courses provided data (response rate = 36.5%). The number of participants per course ranged between 1 and 33 (M = 5.14, SD = 4.17). In total, 72% of the participants were women, and the mean age was M = 39.73 (SD = 11.55). Different job positions in the sample included judges (25.8%), public prosecutors (12.4%), legal assistants (45.2%), managers (2.9%) and others (13.6%).

At T2, N = 325 participants from 86 courses filled in the questionnaire (response rate = 23%). Per course, the number of participants ranged between 1 and 18 (M = 3.78, SD = 2.74). From the total number of participants at T2, N = 180 also filled in a questionnaire at T1.

Measures

To measure the UMTM components at both time points, the questionnaire of De Brabander and Glastra (2018a) was adapted to fit the purpose of the current study (see Table 1 for details). All items were answered on a bipolar seven-point Likert answering scale.

Upon inspection of response distributions, the indicators measuring personal and non-personal negative cognitive valence at both time points showed large overrepresentations of values 4 (the middle category) and 7 (negligible). Because of the consequential lack of variance in these variables, we had convergence issues when both indicators were added in the analysis. As such, they were dropped from the study.

The indicators for positive cognitive valences were answered in relation to participants themselves (personal) and their team, court, judicature in general and litigants (non-personal). Moreover, feasibility appraisal consisted of an item measuring sense of personal competence and an item regarding perceived external support. We performed a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) to investigate the factor structure of the nonpersonal cognitive valence and feasibility appraisal. We used multiple goodness-of-fit indicators to investigate model-fit of the CFA. Threshold values require RMSEA to be smaller than 0.06, CFI to approach 0.95 and SRMR to be smaller than 0.08 (Hu & Bentler, 1999). The CFA showed that a model in which indicators loaded on their respective factors fitted well to the data, χ2(7) = 9.48, p = 0.22; RMSEA = 0.02 [0.00; 0.06]; CFI = 0.99, SRMR = 0.02. In this model, the error terms of the positive cognitive valences in relation to the judiciary and litigants were correlated to reach acceptable fit.

The other components of the UMTM were measured with one indicator per component. It is not standard within social sciences to use one item per construct. However, shortening the questionnaire makes it more appealing for practitioners to administer questionnaires (Gogol et al., 2014). Investigating reliability of one-item constructs requires an alternative assessment, which can be done by means of SEM model-fit coefficients. Originally, model-fit coefficients were introduced as a mean to evaluate the reliability of latent structural equated scores (Tucker & Lewis, 1973) and “to avoid models with superfluous parameters that assume meaningless values” (Browne & Cudeck, 1993, p. 136). As unreliable item response patterns are not able to predict or correlate with responses of other items, model-fit indices can provide information regarding the unreliability of item response patterns. In addition, our interest lies in the evaluation of the predictive value of latent structurally equated scores. Unlike regular alpha coefficients, model-fit coefficients do precisely inform about the reliability of these latent scores.

To investigate reliability of the questionnaire representing the UMTM components, we made a path model that contained the UMTM components measured with one item as well as the final structure of the latent variables. All components were related to each other in accordance with the dynamics of the UMTM. In this model, we constrained relationships between the components in the direction that was proposed by De Brabander & Martens (2014). Moreover, we allowed correlations among all types of valences, among all personal predictors of these valences and among their contextual antecedents (cf. De Brabander & Glastra, 2018a). Our results showed that the model fitted well to the data, χ2(40) = 195.19, p < 0.001; RMSEA = 0.08 [0.07; 0.09]; CFI = 0.94, SRMR = 0.05. This provides support for the reliability of the questionnaire used to measure the UMTM components.

To measure transfer of training, at T2 participants were asked whether they had experienced opportunities to apply the course content in practice. In total, 67% of the participants confirmed (N = 247). These participants were asked, additionally, with one item to what extent they had actually applied the course content (i.e., transfer of training) on a five-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (very little) to 5 (very much). To measure transfer, a distinction can be made between transfer quantity and transfer quality (Grohmann et al., 2014). Transfer quantity refers to the frequency to which training content is applied in practice, whereas transfer quality describes the extent to which training content is applied successfully (Grohmann et al., 2014). In this study, we measured transfer quantity. Grohmann, Beller, and Kauffeld (2014) found that transfer motivation was a predictor of transfer quantity regardless of the amount of transfer motivation among trainees, whereas the predictive value of transfer motivation on transfer quality depended on the value of transfer motivation. As such, measuring transfer quantity was deemed to be a better starting point to investigate the predictive value of the UMTM. Participants who had not experienced opportunities to apply the course content were not able to answer the transfer of training item. Therefore, they received the question measuring transfer intention again.

Missing data handling

The total percentage of missing data at T1 and T2 was 37.90%. Without missingness that could be attributed to attrition, this percentage is 13.89%. Without missingness that could be attributed to accretion, the missing data is 2.89%. To assess the nature of missing data due to attrition and accretion, two logistic regression analyses were performed. In the first, all variables at T1 were used to predict whether participants belonged to the group that only provided data for T1 or the group that provided data at both time points. This model did not fit the data better than the baseline model, χ2(15) = 12.84, p = 0.642, which indicates that missing data at T1 did not depend on variables under study. The second logistic regression used the variables at T2 to predict whether participants belonged to the group that only provided data for T2 or the complete data group. This model did improve on the baseline model significantly, χ2(13) = 29.13, p = .006. As such, group membership at T2 depended on variables in the data set and missing data cannot be considered completely at random (Schafer & Graham, 2002). However, because participants indicated that they could not fill in the second measurement because of external reasons (e.g., the invitation getting lost in their email or having other work duties), we argue that data can still be considered missing at random (Schafer & Graham, 2002) and can be imputed using multiple imputation.

Multiple imputation is a state of the art technique to handle missing data (Schafer & Graham, 2002). Data were imputed in SPSS using the fully conditional specification algorithm (Enders, 2010, p. 275). The imputation model included all indicators from T1 and T2, as well as the age and gender of the participants. As such, transfer of training at T2 was also imputed, even though respondents had not had the possibility to transfer training content to the workplace. Likewise, transfer intention at T2 was imputed, even though participants had actually reported to have applied the training content in practice. Prior to imputation, convergence of the imputation algorithm was diagnosed using Enders’ SPSS macro for creating trace plots and the potential scale reduction factor (retrieved from: www.appliedmissingdata.com/macro-programs.html). These diagnostics showed that the imputation model converged and produced independent imputations after 650 iterations. To ascertain independent imputations, the final 50 data sets were imputed using a conservative 1,000 burn-in and 1,000 between-imputation iterations.

Analyses

Data that are collected among individuals nested in groups are typically analyzed using multilevel analysis (Hox, 2010). However, to do so requires a certain number of participants per group to achieve enough power. If researchers are interested in explaining group differences in intercepts or slopes, 100 groups with at least ten participants are recommended. In the current study, participants came from 100 groups (i.e., different courses included in the study). However, 88 groups contained fewer than ten participants. Therefore, the data collected for this study was not considered suitable for multilevel analysis.

Data were analyzed using structural equation modelling in Mplus 7.31 (Muthén & Muthén, 2012), using the previously described 50 imputed data sets. Two models were evaluated. To analyze the extent to which the UMTM predicts transfer of training (research question one), a similar model as for the reliability analysis was used. Moreover, transfer of training was added to this model. In this model, the indicators of T1 were related to each other in accordance with the UMTM and served as predictors of transfer of training at T2. The second model evaluated how changes in UMTM components are related to each other (research question two). For this model, difference scores between indicators at T1 and T2 were calculated. These difference scores were modelled in accordance with the UMTM and used to predict transfer intention and transfer of training at T2. Both transfer intention and transfer of training were included as outcomes because participants who had not gone through transfer opportunities only reported on their transfer intention at T2.

Results

Descriptive statistics

Tables 2 and 3 show means and standard deviations of and correlations between all indicators from T1 and T2 and their difference scores, respectively. From Table 2, it appears that all indicators except transfer intention decreased in the three weeks between T1 and T2. However, paired sample t-tests showed that these changes are only significant for personal sense of autonomy, personal sense of competence and positive cognitive valence. Moreover, Table 3 shows that most correlations were in line with our hypotheses. Notable exceptions in this respect are that only non-personal positive cognitive valence at T1 seemed to be related to transfer intention at T2. Transfer of training, however, was related to most UMTM components at T1. Regarding the correlations between difference scores, it is notable that changes in none of the UMTM components were related to changes in transfer intention. One exception is change in negative affective valence, which was negatively related to change in transfer intention as expected.

The relationship between unified model of task-specific motivation components and transfer of training

To answer our first research question, a path model that was similar to the model with which we investigated reliability was used to examine the relationships between the UMTM components. This model was similar to the path models used in previous UMTM research (De Brabander & Glastra, 2018a; de Brabander & Martens, 2018b; De Brabander & Glastra, 2021). Unlike previous UMTM studies, however, we added transfer of training to the model to investigate the value of the UMTM components in predicting transfer of training at T2. Model-fit was still sufficient to good after adding transfer of training χ2(52) = 180.71, p < 0.001; RMSEA = 0.06 [0.05; 0.07]; CFI = 0.94, SRMR = 0.07. Figure 6 shows that transfer intention at T1 positively predicts transfer of training at T2 with an explained variance of 6% in transfer of training. Transfer intention, in turn, is positively predicted by only non-personal positive cognitive valences. Transfer intention is also directly positively predicted by feasibility appraisal and subjective norm. Whereas feasibility appraisal indirectly predicts transfer intention through non-personal positive cognitive valences as well, subjective norm is only directly related to transfer intention. Sense of personal autonomy is related to all types of valences in line with the hypothesized model, except for non-personal positive cognitive valence. Finally, sense of personal autonomy is positively predicted by perceived freedom of action. Taken together, these results indicate that most antecedents predict the valences and that non-personal positive cognitive valences predict transfer intention, which eventually predicts transfer of training. Moreover, feasibility appraisal and subjective norm also directly predict transfer intention. These outcomes are mostly in line with our hypotheses.

Relationships between changes in unified model of task-specific motivation components

To answer our second research question, difference scores of all indicators measured at T1 and T2 were modelled according to the UMTM to predict transfer intention and transfer of training at T2. There are a few differences between the analyses used to answer our first question (see Figure 6) and for the analysis of change. Firstly, since participants reported either transfer intention or transfer of training at T2, affective and cognitive valence were predictors of both transfer intention and transfer of training. Secondly, for feasibility appraisal, we used a composite measure of sense of personal competence and perceived external support by calculating their average.

A first model, that included all relationships depicted in Figure 1, did not fit well to the data, χ2(9) = 32.29, p < 0.001; RMSEA = 0.06 [0.04; 0.09]; CFI = 0.83, SRMR = 0.03. Because direct effects of feasibility appraisal and subjective norm on transfer intention and/or transfer of training are only considered a possibility by the UMTM, as indicated by the dashed lines in Figure 1, these relationships were omitted, and model-fit was re-evaluated. The resulting model is displayed in Figure 7 and showed good fit with the data, χ2(13) = 22.27, p = 0.05; RMSEA = 0.03 [0.00; 0.06]; CFI = 0.93, SRMR = 0.04.

Figure 7 shows that transfer intention and transfer of training are positively predicted by changes in personal positive cognitive valences with an explained variance in transfer of training of 7%. This association implies that an increase in personal positive cognitive valences is associated with higher transfer intention and transfer of training. It should be noted that the relationship between personal positive cognitive valence and transfer of training was marginally significant, with a p-value of 0.056. Changes in positive cognitive valence, in turn, were positively predicted by changes in feasibility appraisal and subjective norm. Changes in feasibility appraisal also positively predicted changes in non-personal positive cognitive valence. Moreover, changes in subjective norm also positively predicted positive personal cognitive valence and positive affective valence. Finally, changes in sense of personal autonomy were related positively to changes in positive affective valence and negatively to negative affective valence, in line with the UMTM. To sum up, our results provide evidence that changes in transfer intention and transfer of training can be predicted through changes in other UMTM components, in line with our hypothesis.

Discussion

This study tested the adequacy of the UMTM for predicting transfer of training. This was done because transfer of training remains elusory (Botke et al., 2018). The UMTM can inform transfer of training models by providing an explanation for why factors in the work environment are related to transfer intention and, subsequently, transfer of training. However, to date, the UMTM was not employed to predict transfer of training. In addition, existing studies evaluating the UMTM did not investigate changes in components of the model and their relationships. Neither did existing research allow participants to engage in the behaviour for which their motivation was measured to evaluate the predictive value of the model. As such, this study also provides a next step in evaluating the merits of the UMTM in general.

Results showed that the relationships among UMTM components are largely in line with the model and that, through the dynamics of the UMTM, transfer of training can be predicted over time. This conclusion is corroborated by our findings regarding the relationship between changes in UMTM components and the prediction of transfer of training through those changes. In sum, (changes in) most components of the model are directly or indirectly associated with transfer intention and, subsequently, transfer of training. Our outcomes are therewith mostly in line with our hypotheses.

Unlike previous research (De Brabander & Glastra, 2018a; de Brabander & Martens, 2018b; De Brabander & Glastra, 2021), this study found a very central role of feasibility appraisal. This is in line with the studies of Massenberg, Schulte, and Kauffeld (2017) and Salamon et al. (2022), who also found self-efficacy and support at the work floor predict transfer motivation. On the other hand, only a few effects were found for sense of personal autonomy, which is not in line with the study of Colquitt (2000), who found that perceived autonomy was a predictor of transfer motivation. These results may be explained by the fact that employees in the Dutch judiciary tend to have a high professional autonomy (e.g., judges are required to make independent judgements and are appointed for life; Taal, 2016). As such, freedom of action and personal autonomy may be treated in this particular context as given conditions instead of a variable in their motivation to apply training content. This interpretation underscores the conclusions of previous research that the specific dynamics of UMTM components are task and context dependent. Also, in contrast with previous studies, we found no relationship between affective valences and personal positive cognitive valences and transfer intention when controlling for nonpersonal cognitive valences. This implies that employees within the judiciary are primarily motivated by non-personal benefits resulting from applying the training content in practice. This is in line with the work motives of judges. Research showed that the main motivation of judges to work as a judge is to pursue the interests of the public service (i.e., fair and impartial application of the law) and not necessarily to satisfy personal interests (Gomes et al., 2016; Stout, 2002).

Notwithstanding the disparity between current results and previous research, our findings provide the first initial evidence for the predictive value of the UMTM for actual performance of task-specific action. Moreover, by showing that the model works in another context than the teacher professionalization context and that changes in many components of the UMTM are related in accordance with its theoretical assumptions, this study extends earlier cross-sectional research (De Brabander & Glastra, 2018a; de Brabander & Martens, 2018b; De Brabander & Glastra, 2021). In this study, task-specific action was operationalized as transfer quantity as opposed to transfer quality. As previous research (Grohmann et al., 2014) indicated that the link between transfer motivation and transfer quantity and quality works differently, it could be interesting for future research to investigate to what extent the UMTM also is able to predict transfer quality or to investigate differences in the value of the UMTM in predicting transfer quality and transfer quantity.

Furthermore, the analysis of change in this study showed a decrease in sense of personal autonomy, sense of personal competence and personal positive cognitive valence after having gone through action opportunities. Participants’ perceptions of the contexts in which they applied training content thus remained rather stable. These results could suggest that merely going through action opportunities changes employees’ transfer motivation. Thus, experiencing what it means to transfer training to work practice can cause individuals to make more realistic assessments of the volitional nature of their behaviour, their ability to perform that behaviour successfully and the value of doing so. This is in line with the study of Huang, Ford, and Ryan (2017), who examined transfer of training once a week for six weeks and provided evidence for the emergence of transfer of training trajectories in which trainees differ in how transfer of training develops over time. These trajectories might be explained by changes in the personal and contextual antecedents and types of transfer motivation over time that eventually might result in changes in transfer of training as well.

Results of this study would suggest a feedback loop in the UMTM for these components, at least in the context of motivation to transfer training. The theory of planned behaviour accounts for the possibility of such a feedback loop by including the concept of “actual behavioural control”, which refers to individuals’ actual skills and abilities and the presence of actual environmental constraints and facilitators over and above the perceptions of individuals (Fishbein & Ajzen, 2010, p. 21). Actual behavioural control determines the strength of the relationship between behavioural intention and actual behaviour. Moreover, actual behavioural control feeds back into the system of personal and environmental factors that the UMTM adopts from theory of planned behaviour to determine motivated behaviour. It would, therefore, be interesting for future research to further investigate whether a feedback loop exists in the UMTM as a consequence of going through action opportunities, also in other contexts than the one under study in this paper.

For transfer of training models, the effect of valences on transfer intention underscores the necessity to approach transfer motivation as a multidimensional construct (Jacot et al., 2015; Gegenfurtner, 2013; Gegenfurtner et al., 2009). Transfer of training models can also learn from this study that the effects of contextual factors on transfer motivation can be explained via personal experiences of feasibility. This explanatory mechanism can refine theorizing about factors that may influence transfer of training. It may, for example, explain results regarding the effect of performance feedback on transfer motivation, which is inconsistent (cf. Kirwan & Birchall, 2006; Massenberg et al., 2017). Transfer of training models can learn from self-determination theory assumptions, which are extended into the UMTM, that only certain types of feedback have competence-enhancing and motivating effects, which may explain these inconsistent findings. It would be interesting for future research to investigate whether the effects of specific contextual factors that are known to influence transfer motivation are present because they contribute to employees’ feasibility appraisals. The UMTM provides a framework for theorizing how these factors may be interrelated in their effects.

Limitations and directions for future research

Some limitations should be acknowledged. Firstly, it should be noted that the amount of explained variance of transfer of training through the dynamics of the UMTM is rather low. This implies that future studies should include other factors that influence transfer of training, especially in terms of training characteristics. Such a factor could include, for example, the quality of the training, which has been shown to predict transfer of training (Grossman & Salas, 2011).

Secondly, this study’s measures for negative cognitive valences could not be used in our analyses. This was caused by large overrepresentations of the most negative and neutral response options in these measures, which indicated that questions were either not understood well or could not distinguish between participants. In hindsight, the reason for this may have been that participants could not imagine “costs or unwanted consequences” of applying the course content for themselves or others. Therefore, future research should provide examples of what such costs of unwanted consequences could be.

Thirdly, the current assessment of transfer of training concerned a self-report measure. As such, effects of UMTM components were found on participants’ subjective experiences of having applied training content in the work context. Although subjective experiences are the only valid source of information regarding motivational factors from the UMTM, subjective experiences of transfer of training are known to differ from ratings by thirds (Blume et al., 2010). Moreover, using self-reports can lead to common method bias. More insight into the predictive value of the UMTM for predicting transfer of training would, therefore, require intersubjective measures of transfer, like the ratings of trainers or supervisors on whether the content is being used in practice, independently of employees’ own experiences.

Practical implications

We have several practical recommendations that can be used by organizations and training institutes to raise transfer of training. It is important that trainers and policymakers focus on raising sense of personal competence, perceived external support and subjective norm. Sense of personal competence could be raised by setting goals at the end of trainings that aim at what one wants to achieve through applying training content (Brown & Warren, 2009). Moreover, perceived external support could be fostered by ensuring that trainees receive sufficient support from colleagues in applying training content. For example, supervisors could encourage trainees to apply training content, and they could give frequent feedback about the application of training content in practice (Grossman & Salas, 2011). Furthermore, subjective norm could be enhanced by letting multiple supervisors and peers of the same organization take part in the training. This can increase the likelihood that the content of training aligns with the norms of the organization (Gilpin-Jackson & Bushe, 2007). In addition, this can increase positive attitudes among colleagues regarding the application of the training content (Salamon et al., 2022).

We also recommend trainers and practitioners to implement the questionnaire representing the UMTM to examine the extent to which training content is used in practice at the end of trainings. Our results underline that the questionnaire has a value in predicting transfer of training and shows which personal and contextual factors could be improved to raise transfer of training. Moreover, the survey is relatively short. Administering short questionnaires can enhance the response rate of the questionnaire (Gogol et al., 2014). As such, this questionnaire can be a practical instrument for organizations to acquire more insight into the applications of trainings in practice.

Together, the recommendations inferred from the results of our study can provide organizations and training institutes with a tool that can be used to monitor transfer of training and inform measures aiming to raise transfer of training. Ultimately, this can lead to increasing the desired impact of trainings on organizational practice and provide a better return on all the investments made in trainings.

Figures

Unified model of task-specific motivation

Figure 1.

Unified model of task-specific motivation

Expected relationships between affective and cognitive valences, readiness for action and action

Figure 2.

Expected relationships between affective and cognitive valences, readiness for action and action

Expected relationships between feasibility appraisal and affective and cognitive valences

Figure 3.

Expected relationships between feasibility appraisal and affective and cognitive valences

Expected relationships between sense of perceived freedom of action, sense of personal autonomy and affective and cognitive valence

Figure 4.

Expected relationships between sense of perceived freedom of action, sense of personal autonomy and affective and cognitive valence

Expected relationships between subjective norm, sense of personal relatedness and affective and cognitive valence

Figure 5.

Expected relationships between subjective norm, sense of personal relatedness and affective and cognitive valence

Standardized relationships between UMTM indicators and transfer of training. For clarity of the figure, non-significant relationships, correlations between indicators and error terms were omitted. Personal cognitive valence is indicated by (p). Non-personal cognitive valence is indicated by (np).

Figure 6.

Standardized relationships between UMTM indicators and transfer of training. For clarity of the figure, non-significant relationships, correlations between indicators and error terms were omitted. Personal cognitive valence is indicated by (p). Non-personal cognitive valence is indicated by (np).

Standardized relationships between difference scores of UMTM components, transfer intention and transfer of training. For clarity of the figure, non-significant relationships, correlations between indicators, and error terms were omitteda. Personal cognitive valence is indicated by (p). Non-personal cognitive valence is indicated by (np). † indicates that p = .056

Figure 7.

Standardized relationships between difference scores of UMTM components, transfer intention and transfer of training. For clarity of the figure, non-significant relationships, correlations between indicators, and error terms were omitteda. Personal cognitive valence is indicated by (p). Non-personal cognitive valence is indicated by (np). † indicates that p = .056

Indicators and answering scales for UMTM components included in this study

Component Item Answering scale
1. Perceived freedom of action When putting the things that were offered in this course into practice, I will have […] opportunities for free choice Very much – Very little
2. Sense of personal autonomy When applying this course’s content in my job, I would feel I did so […] Completely out of my own volition – Completely out of experienced pressure
3. Perceived external support I find the facilities in our court to apply what I have learned successfully […] Very obstructive – Very conducive
4. Sense of personal competence I personally feel […] to successfully apply the knowledge, skills and insights that I acquired in this course Very able – Not able at all
5. Subjective norm I think that colleagues who are important to me would assess me applying what I have learned during the courses Not positive at all – Very positive
6. Positive affective valence When applying the knowledge, skills and insights that I acquired in this course, I would […] have a positive feeling Very often – Rarely or never
7. Negative affective valence When applying the knowledge, skills and insights that I acquired in this course, I would […] have a negative feeling Rarely or never – Very often
8. Positive cognitive valence Considering the positive consequences, applying the course content in my job would be […] Not or hardly rewarding – Very rewarding
9. Negative cognitive valence The costs and unwanted consequences of applying the course content in my job would be […] Very heavy – Negligible
10. Transfer intention I am going to apply the things that I have learned during the course in my job Completely disagree – Completely agree
11. Transfer of training* To what extent did you put the learned content into practice in the written document that you handed in? None – Very much
Notes:

Items 1, 2, 4, 6 and 9 were recoded so that a high value would indicate much of the measured construct; *item was only measured after three weeks

Source: Jansen in de Wal et al.

Means and standard deviations of all measures at T1 and T2 and their difference scores

T1 (N = 514) T2 (N = 325) T2−T1 (N = 180)
Component M SD M SD M SD
Perceived freedom of action 5.21 1.43 5.13 1.86 −0.08 2.21
Sense of personal autonomy 5.91 1.32 5.76 1.68 −0.16* 1.80
Perceived external support 4.81 1.40 4.74 1.73 −0.06 2.02
Sense of personal competence 5.61 1.18 5.40 1.58 −0.21** 1.78
Subjective norm 5.20 1.57 5.16 1.81 −0.04 2.15
Positive affective valence 5.49 1.07 5.39 1.32 −0.10 1.52
Negative affective valence 2.23 1.45 2.10 1.80 −0.13 2.16
Positive cognitive valence (p) 5.65 1.20 5.38 1.62 −0.27*** 1.67
Positive cognitive valence (np) 5.29 1.15 5.19 1.47 −0.11 1.51
Transfer intention 5.49 1.94 5.60 5.76
Transfer of training 3.85 1.44
Notes:

Personal cognitive valence is indicated by (p). Non-personal cognitive valence is indicated by (np); *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001

Source: Jansen in de Wal et al.

Correlations between all measures at T1 and T2 and their difference scores

Component 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. 20.
1. Perceived freedom of action T1
2. Sense of personal autonomy T1 0.25***
3. Perceived external support T1 0.09 0.08
4. Sense of personal competence T1 0.23*** 0.21*** 0.17***
5. Subjective norm T1 0.10* 0.20*** 0.34*** 0.25***
6. Positive affective valence T1 0.27*** 0.33*** 0.12** 0.33*** 0.30***
7. Negative affective valence T1 −0.11* −0.30*** −0.13** −0.26*** −0.20*** −0.39***
8. Positive cognitive valence (p) T1 0.17*** 0.28*** 0.23*** 0.36*** 0.29*** 0.42*** −0.32***
9. Positive cognitive valence (np) T1 0.03 0.16*** 0.21*** 0.34*** 0.27*** 0.40*** −0.31*** 0.67***
10. Transfer intention T1 0.10* 0.18*** 0.33*** 0.31*** 0.41*** 0.28*** −0.26*** 0.44*** 0.43***
11. Perceived freedom of action T2 0.39*** 0.31*** 0.07 0.33*** 0.18** 0.19** −0.12* 0.15* 0.11 0.08 0.12 −0.05 −0.08 −0.11 0.15* −0.05 −0.03 −0.02 −0.00
12. Sense of personal autonomy T2 0.22** 0.53*** 0.17* 0.24** 0.18** 0.26*** −0.27*** 0.36*** 0.26*** 0.14* 0.40*** −0.01 0.03 0.15 0.21* −0.23** −0.05 0.01 0.11
13. Perceived external support T2 0.13* 0.00 0.40*** 0.13 0.20** 0.13 −0.12 0.23** 0.13* 0.20** 0.06 0.09 0.11 0.25*** 0.06 0.03 0.16* 0.24** 0.04
14. Sense of personal competence T2 0.24*** 0.27*** 0.13 0.36*** 0.25*** 0.25*** −0.16* 0.36*** 0.38*** 0.31*** 0.23*** 0.32*** 0.22*** 0.13 0.13 −0.21** 0.18* 0.22** 0.17
15. Subjective norm T2 0.23*** 0.16* 0.29*** 0.27*** 0.37*** 0.25*** −0.22** 0.37*** 0.35*** 0.31*** 0.17** 0.30*** 0.45*** 0.42*** 0.21** −0.13 0.19* 0.16* 0.07
16. Positive affective valence T2 0.18** 0.26*** 0.19** 0.40*** 0.24*** 0.44*** −0.24*** 0.38*** 0.42*** 0.29*** 0.28*** 0.40*** 0.26*** 0.46*** 0.42*** −0.30*** 0.24** 0.25*** 0.19
17. Negative affective valence T2 −0.16** −0.20** −0.19** −0.17* −0.06 −0.24** 0.29*** −0.15* −0.20** −0.08 −0.23*** −0.42*** −0.14* −0.34*** −0.26*** −0.46*** −0.17* −0.11 −0.34***
18. Positive cognitive valence (p) T2 0.19** 0.25*** 0.14* 0.34*** 0.19* 0.38*** −0.25*** 0.51*** 0.54*** 0.34*** 0.15** 0.27*** 0.29*** 0.50*** 0.44*** 0.57*** −0.30*** 0.59*** 0.20
19. Positive cognitive valence (np) T2 0.06 0.13* 0.13* 0.24** 0.22** 0.28*** −0.26*** 0.40*** 0.61*** 0.28*** 0.10 0.23*** 0.28*** 0.48*** 0.44*** 0.52*** −0.27*** 0.78*** 0.15
20. Transfer intention T2 0.08 0.09 0.15 0.16 0.16 0.09 −0.09 0.22 0.27* 0.20 0.07 0.17 0.10 0.36*** 0.17* 0.32** −0.40*** 0.37*** 0.30**
21. Transfer of training T2 0.13* 0.13 0.18* 0.30*** 0.10 0.16* −0.10 0.35*** 0.31*** 0.24** 0.09 0.09 0.16** 0.39*** 0.30*** 0.29*** −0.16** 0.48*** 0.42*** 0.44**
Notes:

Correlations between difference scores are presented above the diagonal. Personal cognitive valence is indicated by (p). Non-personal cognitive valence is indicated by (np). *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001

Source: Jansen in de Wal et al.

References

Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (2008). Scaling and testing multiplicative combinations in the expectancy–value model of attitudes. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 38(9), 22222247, doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2008.00389.x.

Beer, M., Finnstrom, M., & Schrader, D. (2016). The great training robbery. Harvard Business School Research Paper Series, 16121, doi: 10.2139/ssrn.2759357.

Blume, B. D., Ford, J. K., Baldwin, T. T., & Huang, J. L. (2010). Transfer of training: a meta-analytic review. Journal of Management, 36(4), 10651105, doi: 10.1177/0149206309352880.

Botke, J. A., Jansen, P. G., Khapova, S. N., & Tims, M. (2018). Work factors influencing the transfer stages of soft skills training: a literature review. Educational Research Review, 24, 130147, doi: 10.1016/j.edurev.2018.04.001.

Brown, T. C., & Warren, A. M. (2009). Distal goal and proximal goal transfer of training interventions in an executive education program. Human Resource Development Quarterly, 20(3), 265284, doi: 10.1002/hrdq.20021.

Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. Sociological Methods & Research, 21(2), 136162, doi: 10.1177/0049124192021002005.

Colquitt, J. A., LePine, J. A., & Noe, R. A. (2000). Toward an integrative theory of training motivation: a meta-analytic path analysis of 20 years of research. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85(5), 678707, doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.85.5.678.

Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: the psychology of optimal experience, New York, NY: Harper & Row.

De Brabander, C. J., & Martens, R. L. (2014). Towards a unified theory of task-specific motivation. Educational Research Review, 11, 2744, doi: 10.1016/j.edurev.2013.11.001.

De Brabander, C. J., & Glastra, F. J. (2018a). Testing a unified model of task-specific motivation: How teachers appraise three professional development activities. Frontline Learning Research, 6(1), 5476. doi: 10.14786/flr.v6i1.342.

de Brabander, C. J., & Martens, R. L. (2018b). Empirical exploration of a unified model of task-specific motivation. Psychology, 9(4), 540560, doi: 10.4236/psych.2018.94033.

De Brabander, C. J., & Glastra, F. J. (2021). The unified model of task-specific motivation and teachers’ motivation to learn about teaching and learning supportive modes of ICT use. Education and Information Technologies, 26, 393420, doi: 10.1007/s10639-020-10256-7.

Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11(4), 227268, doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli1104_01.

Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational beliefs, values, and goals. Annual Review of Psychology, 53(1), 109132, doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135153.

Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2020). From expectancy-value theory to situated expectancy-value theory: A developmental, social cognitive, and sociocultural perspective on motivation. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 61, 101859, doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2020.101859.

Elliot, A. J. (2006). The hierarchical model of approach-avoidance motivation. Motivation and Emotion, 30(2), 111116, doi: 10.1007/s11031-006-9028-7.

Enders, C. K. (2010). Applied missing data analysis, New York, NY: Guilford press.

Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (2010). Predicting and changing behavior, New York, NY: Taylor and Francis Group.

Ford, J. K., Baldwin, T. T., & Prasad, J. (2018). Transfer of training: the known and the unknown. Annual Review of Organizational Psychology and Organizational Behavior, 5(1), 201225, doi: 10.1146/annurev-orgpsych-032117-104443.

Gegenfurtner, A. (2013). Dimensions of motivation to transfer: a longitudinal analysis of their influence on retention, transfer, and attitude change. Vocations and Learning, 6(2), 187205, doi: 10.1007/s12186-012-9084-y.

Gegenfurtner, A., Festner, D., Gallenberger, W., Lehtinen, E., & Gruber, H. (2009). Predicting autonomous and controlled motivation to transfer training. International Journal of Training and Development, 13(2), 124138, doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2 419.2009.00322.x.

Gegenfurtner, A., Könings, K. D., Kosmajac, N., & Gebhardt, M. (2016). Voluntary or mandatory training participation as a moderator in the relationship between goal orientations and transfer of training. International Journal of Training and Development, 20(4), 290301, doi: 10.1111/ijtd.12089.

Gegenfurtner, A., & Quesada-Pallarès, C. (2022). Toward a multidimensional conceptualization of motivation to transfer training: Validation of the transfer motivation questionnaire from a self-determination theory perspective using bifactor-ESEM. Studies in Educational Evaluation, 73, 101116.

Gegenfurtner, A., & Testers, L. (2022). Transfer of training among non-traditional students in higher education: Testing the theory of planned behavior. Frontiers in Education, 7, 17, doi: 10.3389/feduc.2022.928996.

Gilpin-Jackson, Y., & Bushe, G. R. (2007). Leadership development training transfer: A case study of post-training determinants. Journal of Management Development, 26(10), 9801004, doi: 10.1108/02621710710833423.

Gogol, K., Brunner, M., Goetz, T., Martin, R., Ugen, S., Keller, U., … Preckel, F. (2014). “My questionnaire is too long!” the assessments of motivational-affective constructs with three-item and single-item measures. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 39(3), 188205, doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2014.04.002.

Gomes, A. O., Guimaraes, T. A., & de Souza, E. C. L. (2016). Judicial work and judges’ motivation: The perceptions of Brazilian state judges. Law & Policy, 38(2), 162176, doi: 10.1111/lapo.12050.

Grohmann, A., Beller, J., & Kauffeld, S. (2014). Exploring the critical role of motivation to transfer in the training transfer process. International Journal of Training and Development, 18(2), 84103, doi: 10.1111/ijtd.12030.

Grossman, R., & Salas, E. (2011). The transfer of training: what really matters. International Journal of Training and Development, 15(2), 103120, doi: 10.1111/j.1468- 2419.2011.00373.x.

Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., Pintrich, P. R., Elliot, A. J., & Thrash, T. M. (2002). Revision of achievement goal theory: Necessary and illuminating. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94(3), 638645, doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.94.3.638.

Holton, E. F. (1996). The flawed four‐level evaluation model. Human Resource Development Quarterly, 7(1), 521, doi: 10.1002/hrdq.3920070103.

Hox, J. J. (2010). Multilevel analysis: Techniques and applications, New York, NY: Routledge.

Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 155, doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118.

Huang, J. L., Ford, J. K., & Ryan, A. M. (2017). Ignored no more: Within‐person variability enables better understanding of training transfer. Personnel Psychology, 70(3), 557596, doi: 10.1111/peps.12155.

Jacot, A., Raemdonck, I., & Frenay, M. (2015). A review of motivational constructs in learning and training transfer. Zeitschrift für Erziehungswissenschaft, 18(S1), 201219, doi: 10.1007/s11618-014-0599-x.

Kirwan, C., & Birchall, D. (2006). Transfer of learning from management development programmes: Testing the Holton model. International Journal of Training and Development, 10(4), 252268, doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2419.2006.00259.x.

Krapp, A. (2002). Structural and dynamic aspects of interest development: Theoretical considerations from an ontogenetic perspective. Learning and Instruction, 12(4), 383409, doi: 10.1016/s0959-4752%2801%2900011-1.

Massenberg, A. C., Schulte, E. M., & Kauffeld, S. (2017). Never too early: Learning transfer system factors affecting motivation to transfer before and after training programs. Human Resource Development Quarterly, 28(1), 5585, doi: 10.1002/hrdq.21256.

Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2012). Mplus 7, Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.

Poell, R. F. (2017). Time to ‘flip’ the training transfer tradition: Employees create learning paths strategically. Human Resource Development Quarterly, 28(1), 915, doi: 10.1002/hrdq.21279.

Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2020). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation from a self-determination theory perspective: Definitions, theory, practices, and future directions. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 61, 101860, doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2020.101860.

Salamon, J., Blume, B. D., Tóth‐Király, I., Nagy, T., & Orosz, G. (2022). The positive gain spiral of job resources, work engagement, opportunity and motivation on training transfer. International Journal of Training and Development, 26(3), 556580, doi: 10.1111/ijtd.12277.

Schafer, J. L., & Graham, J. W. (2002). Missing data: Our view of the state of the art. Psychological Methods, 7(2), 147177, doi: 10.1037/1082-989x.7.2.147.

Schunk, D. H., & DiBenedetto, M. K. (2020). Motivation and social cognitive theory. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 60, 101832, doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2019.101832.

Segers, M., & Gegenfurtner, A. (2013). Transfer of training: New conceptualizations through integrated research perspectives. Educational Research Review, 8, 14, doi: 10.1016/j.edurev.2012.11.007.

Stout, L. A. (2002). Judges as altruistic hierarchs. 1605-1628. William & Mary Law Review, 43, doi: 10.2139/ssrn.287458.

Taal, S. (2016). Working separately together: A quantitative study into the knowledge sharing behaviour of judges (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from www.justizforschung.ch/pdf/Taal_Working_separately_together.pdf

Tonhäuser, C., & Büker, L. (2016). Determinants of transfer of training: A comprehensive literature review. International Journal for Research in Vocational Education and Training, 3(2), 127165, doi: 10.13152/IJRVET.3.2.4.

Tucker, L. R., & Lewis, C. (1973). A reliability coefficient for maximum likelihood factor analysis. Psychometrika, 38(1), 110, doi: 10.1007/BF02291170.

Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (2000). Expectancy–value theory of achievement motivation. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25(1), 6881, doi: 10.1006/ceps.1999.1015.

Acknowledgements

Declaration of interest: None

Joost Jansen in de Wal and Bas de Jong share co-first authorship.

Erratum: It has come to the attention of the publisher that the article, Jansen in de Wal, J., de Jong, B., Cornelissen, F. and Brabander, C.d. (2023), “Predicting transfer of training through the unified model of task-specific motivation”, The Learning Organization, 30(6), 834–856. https://doi.org/10.1108/TLO-09-2022-0112, failed to remove anonymised references at proof stage. References throughout to Author et al. (2014) and Author et al. (2018b) have been changed to De Brabander, C. J., & Martens, R. L. (2014). Towards a unified theory of task-specific motivation. Educational Research Review, 11, 27-44. doi: 10.1016/j.edurev.2013.11.001 and de Brabander, C. J., & Martens, R. L. (2018b). Empirical exploration of a unified model of task-specific motivation. Psychology, 9(4), 540-560. doi: 10.4236/psych.2018.94033.

This error was introduced in the editorial process and has now been corrected in the online version. The publisher sincerely apologises for this error and for any inconvenience caused.

Corresponding author

Bas de Jong can be contacted at: b.dejong@uva.nl

Related articles