Understanding behavioral strategy: a historical evolutionary perspective in “Management Decision”

Matteo Cristofaro (Department of Management and Law, University of Rome Tor Vergata, Rome, Italy)
Pier Luigi Giardino (Doctoral School of Social Sciences, University of Trento, Trento, Italy)
Riccardo Camilli (Department of Management and Law, University of Rome Tor Vergata, Rome, Italy)
Ivo Hristov (Department of Management and Law, University of Rome Tor Vergata, Rome, Italy)

Management Decision

ISSN: 0025-1747

Article publication date: 9 September 2024

697

Abstract

Purpose

This article aims to trace the historical development of the behavioral strategy (BS) field, which implements psychology in strategic management. Mainly, it provides a contextual understanding of how this stream of research has historically evolved and what relevant future trajectories are. This work is part of the “over half a century of Management Decision” celebrative and informal Journal section.

Design/methodology/approach

We consider BS literature produced in management decision (MD), the oldest and longest-running scholarly publication in management, as a proxy for the evolution of management thought. Through a Systematic Literature Review (SLR) process, we collected – via the MD website and Scopus – a sample of 97 BS articles published in MD from its foundation (1967) until today (2024). Regarding the analysis, we adopted a Reflexive Thematic Analysis approach to synthesize the main BS topics, then read from a historical perspective regarding three “eras” over which the literature developed. Selected international literature outside the Journal’s boundaries was considered to complement this historical analysis.

Findings

Historically, within the BS field, the interest passed from the rules to rationally govern strategic decision-making processes, to studying what causes cognitive errors, to understanding how to avoid biases and to being prepared for dramatic changes. The article also identifies six future research trajectories, namely “positive heuristics,” “context-embedded mental processes,” “non-conventional thinking,” “cognitive evolutionary triggers,” “debiasing strategies” and “behavioral theories for new strategic challenges” that future research could investigate.

Research limitations/implications

The limitation of the study lies in its exclusive focus on MD for investigating the historical evolution of BS, thereby overlooking critical contributions from other journals. Therefore, MD’s editorial preferences have influenced results. A comprehensive SLR on the BS field is still needed, requiring broader journal coverage to mitigate selection biases and enhance field appraisal.

Originality/value

This contribution is the first to offer a historical evolutionary view of the BS field, complementing the few other reviews on this stream of research. This fills a gap in the study of the evolution of management thought.

Keywords

Citation

Cristofaro, M., Giardino, P.L., Camilli, R. and Hristov, I. (2024), "Understanding behavioral strategy: a historical evolutionary perspective in “Management Decision”", Management Decision, Vol. 62 No. 13, pp. 426-455. https://doi.org/10.1108/MD-01-2023-0072

Publisher

:

Emerald Publishing Limited

Copyright © 2024, Matteo Cristofaro, Pier Luigi Giardino, Riccardo Camilli and Ivo Hristov

License

Published by Emerald Publishing Limited. This article is published under the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY 4.0) licence. Anyone may reproduce, distribute, translate and create derivative works of this article (for both commercial and non-commercial purposes), subject to full attribution to the original publication and authors. The full terms of this licence may be seen at http://creativecommons.org/licences/by/4.0/legalcode


1. Introduction

Significant advancements regarding the psychology of humans in organizations – e.g. bounded rationality (Simon, 1947), dominant coalitions (Cyert and March 1963) and cognitive biases (Tversky and Kahneman, 1974) – have led to a behavioral revolution within and outside management and organizational studies (e.g. Kahneman and Tversky, 1979; Thaler, 1985). Building from them, Powell et al. (2011) strived to define the boundaries of a newly emerging field able to deepen behavioral assumptions (and consequences) in strategic management: Behavioral Strategy (BS) [1]. In particular, they stated that:

Behavioral strategy merges cognitive and social psychology with strategic management theory and practice. The behavioral strategy aims to bring realistic assumptions about human cognition, emotions, and social behavior to the strategic management of organizations and, thereby, to enrich strategy theory, empirical research, and real-world practice (p. 1371).

Since its formalization in 2011 by Powel, Lovallo and Fox, the BS field has been increasingly advanced in studying firms’ strategic practices (Lovallo and Sibony, 2018; Abatecola and Cristofaro, 2020; Foss, 2020). For example, some scholars investigated the concept of CEOs’ hubris, illustrating how it can lead executives to overestimate their firms’ capabilities and performance (Picone et al., 2014). Others tried to unveil how investors and founders often cognitively anchor Unicorn companies’ success expectations to the results of peer organizations, potentially generating overly optimistic expectations about future growth and profitability (Cristofaro et al., 2023). These are just a glimpse of the vast body of literature that has embraced a behavioral approach to unravel the impact of executive psychology in strategic management.

Moreover, as the interest in a research area grows, mapping its literature becomes crucial to help identify trends and pinpoint aspects that still need comprehensive exploration (Tranfield et al., 2003; Edmondson and McManus, 2007). Two bibliometric studies (Anwar et al., 2021; Urío et al., 2022) and a recent literature review (Hesselbarth et al., 2023) have been conducted on the BS field. However, these studies do not provide a thorough historical overview of the field’s evolution or utilize a comprehensive set of keywords for article selection (e.g. Anwar et al., 2021). These limitations can result in neglecting certain key aspects that may have significantly impacted the evolution of the BS field, or they can impede the comprehension of its concepts, perspectives, theories, methodologies and findings. Consequently, the lack of a historical analysis of BS prevents scholars from comprehensively understanding the evolution of management thought (Wren and Bedeian, 2020). It inhibits their ability to forecast the future trajectories of BS research.

To address this gap, we examined the literature published in a Journal that reliably reflects the field’s evolution (see also Gordon et al., 2020). In particular, we performed it in Management Decision (MD), which is acknowledged as “the oldest and longest-running scholarly publication dedicated to the area of management” (Randolph-Seng, 2022, p. 2). Given its enduring legacy as well as its ever-since interest in publishing articles adopting a psychology lens in strategic management (e.g. Tyzack, 1967; Davidson and Cooper, 1987; Bolaños et al., 2005; Elbanna et al., 2017; Al-Shammari et al., 2023), we believe that MD serves as a suitable proxy for analyzing the evolution of management thought regarding BS and its main themes. Therefore, this article addresses the following research questions: (1) How has the field of BS historically evolved in MD? and (2) To what extent has MD influenced the BS debate?

We conducted a Systematic Literature Review (SLR) to answer these research questions. We collected 97 BS articles published in MD along its 57-year history. Then, we analyzed articles through a Reflexive Thematic Analysis (RTA) approach to identify the main themes featuring BS’s historical evolution. The future research agenda also considers other international literature (e.g. Borchardt et al., 2022; Akinci and Sadler-Smith, 2012). In the early stages of BS research (1967–1989), scholars focused on defining decision-making models and prescribing organizational methods to make optimal decisions (e.g. White, 1984). Researchers’ interest expanded to collective executive decision-making in the second era (1990–2011). It explored executives’ abilities, traits, attitudes, culture and biases in choosing decision-making models for improved decisions (e.g. Aspara et al., 2011). Then, in the third era (2012–2024), scholars integrated various theoretical BS concepts to understand the individual level of strategic decision-making (e.g. Ridge et al., 2014). Finally, we present six emerging themes that may drive the research agenda of the next era of BS research, especially in MD.

As a result, this article, while primarily focused on MD, makes three significant contributions to the BS literature. First, it addresses a gap in exploring the evolution of management thought concerning the BS field. Second, it celebrates the new “Over-half a century of Management Decision” section, emphasizing how the journal has actively laid the foundation of BS since the late 1960s. Third, it provides a research agenda for scholars based on state-of-the-art knowledge about BS past and present.

2. Theoretical roots of behavioral strategy

Until the first half of the 20th century, economics and management scholars made their assumptions depending on what was postulated by neoclassical economic theory. In particular, they emphasized the concept of homo economicus, who is expected to: act as a perfectly rational agent, gather and analyze all available information, weigh the costs and benefits of alternatives and make choices that would maximize his outcomes (Thaler, 2000). However, as researchers investigated decision-making processes more comprehensively, they discovered that this neoclassical perspective did not fully capture the complexities of real-world executives’ decision-making (March and Simon, 1958). Indeed, it became evident that apical decision-makers face cognitive limitations, such as information overload and limited computational capacity, that prevent them from fully optimizing their choices, thus rendering the assumption of entirely rational decision-making unrealistic.

In this context, the most distinguished contribution was brought by Herbert Simon, who challenged the prevailing neoclassical perspective in 1947 by introducing the concept of bounded rationality. Specifically, Simon (1947) argued that individuals, to reduce cognitive load, use mental shortcuts and satisfying strategies aimed at simplifying decision-making tasks and arriving at satisfactory rather than optimal outcomes. However, these shortcuts are often based on past experiences, social norms and other contextual factors, leading to cognitive distortions (i.e. biases) and errors in decision-making.

Over the years, this concept of bounded rationality paved the way for many research streams to investigate how decisions realistically occur (e.g. Cristofaro, 2017), mainly based on three philosophical perspectives advanced in the positioning BS article (Powell et al., 2011), i.e. reductionist, pluralist and contextualist. These, by combining elements from cognitive and social psychology with strategic management theory and practice, aim to incorporate practical knowledge about human cognition, emotions and social interactions into organizations’ strategic management at different analysis levels (i.e. individual, collective and contextual) (Powell et al., 2011). Thus, providing a more accurate and holistic representation of decision-making processes in real-world practice. Among produced advancements, it is worth mentioning the Behavioral Theory of the Firm (Cyert and March 1963), the prospect theory (Kahneman and Tversky, 1979), heuristic and biases (Kahneman, 2011), Hambrick and Mason’s (1984) view of organizations as reflections of their top management teams, and Thaler’s (2000) nudge theory and concept of mental accounting. Those formed the foundations of the BS field, allowing to shed light on many critical strategic processes like individual and intragroup decision-making, politics, conflict resolution, organizational learning, resource allocation, sensemaking, perception, enactment and action generation (e.g. Picone et al., 2014).

3. Methods

3.1 Article collection

An SLR has been considered the most appropriate research approach to consolidate and synthesize academic research. According to Tranfield et al. (2003), unlike traditional narrative reviews, an SLR links future research to the questions and concerns posed by past research and employs rigorous and reproducible evaluation methods. In line with those scholars, our review of the historical evolution of the BS field, considering MD literature, has been conducted in two main phases: (1) literature search and selection and (2) literature analysis. The first phase comprises five main steps, described as follows (see also Figure 1).

  1. MD literature search and selection occurred through (a) the MD’s website and (b) an online database (i.e. Scopus). A Scopus search was needed since, on the MD website, conducting a proper Boolean search in the current state is not possible. Using a combination of keywords and Boolean operators (i.e. AND, OR) in Scopus allowed us to ensure that articles of interest were in the final sample.

  2. Following Rabetino et al. (2021), we selected strategy-related articles if they included at least one of the following keywords within their title or abstract: “Strateg*” OR “Competit*” OR “Resource*based View” OR “RBV” OR “Resource*based Theory” OR “Industrial Organization” OR “Competitive Advantage” OR “Governance” OR “Capabilit*” OR “Competit*Dynamic*” OR “Business Model*” OR “Absorptive Capacit*” OR “Capacit*” OR “Resourc*” OR “Analys*” OR “Competitive Intelligence” OR “Upper Echelon*” OR “Decision*” OR “Business Strategies” OR “Knowledge*based Theory” OR “Strategy*as*practice”. The asterisk at the end of a search word allows for different suffixes, such as “Strategic Planning,” “Strategy Implementation,” “Strategy Process,” “Strategy Research,” etc. 891 hits were produced.

  3. Due to the integration operated through a reference-manager software, duplicates from Scopus and manual search within the Journal’s website were eliminated. Yet, we eliminated all articles dealing with entrepreneurial, employee, customer and finance behavior to avoid articles that did not fit (thus overlapping with entrepreneurial behavior, organizational behavior, customer behavior and financial behavior). A total of 468 hits were produced;

  4. The resulting articles were scanned by reading all the titles and abstracts to ensure their substantive context, mainly according to their coherence with the review’s behavioral aim. In the absence of a comprehensive keywords string authoritatively used to trace behavioral themes, such a manual approach was necessary to ensure a faithful tracing of the field’s historical development. Indeed, it required including all articles addressing the same behavioral themes, although the related terminology may have varied. For practically doing that, we were inspired by the key psychological concepts and key psychological processes studies in BS research initially identified by Powell et al. (2011). A total of 211 records remained within the sample.

  5. The remaining articles were fully read to ensure their alignment with the research objective, thus explicitly contributing to the development of strategic management and applying, in conceptual or empirical terms, social and cognitive psychology concepts in their research. A total of 97 results were produced. For steps (4) and (5), two authors individually read articles and compared their evaluations. When disagreeing, these authors assessed articles and decided whether to include or not articles within the sample. Inter-rater reliability, investigated through Cronbach’s alpha, was high (0.86). Moreover, to avoid missing relevant contributions for this review, the two other authors read and evaluated all titles, abstracts and introductions of articles published on MD for potential inclusion, but no additional items emerged.

For each article in the sample, we extracted the following elements: (1) author(s), (2) year of publication, (3) type of article, (4) data collection method, (5) data analysis method, (6) main results, (7) scientific paradigm and (8) investigated key behavioral variables. See Table 1 for a selection of sample articles and Supplementary material for a richer sub-sample.

3.2 Reflexive Thematic Analysis

Investigated key behavioral variables were coded and grouped according to a RTA approach (Braun and Clarke, 2019). This is a valuable approach considered within the analytical step of the SLR process to synthesize and code an identified body of literature to develop insights and critiques (Massaro et al., 2016; Tranfield et al., 2003). When correctly carried out using codes and themes, RTA displays data and information in an excellent, unambiguous way, supporting scholars in defining the theoretical and practical links needed for a deeper comprehension of the mechanisms behind the topic under investigation. RTA usually requires deductive analysis (by which communication messages are thematized according to an initial codebook) and inductive analysis (by which new themes are free to emerge), helping to overcome the limits of each. In particular, the following RTA six-step procedure by Braun and Clarke (2019) has been followed: (1) familiarizing yourself with your data, (2) generating initial codes, (3) searching for themes, (4) reviewing themes, (5) defining and naming themes, and (6) producing the report. In this case, initial codes have been derived from the key psychological concepts and key psychological processes investigated in BS (i.e. Powell et al., 2011). Other codes were free to emerge. Then, themes were identified according to their “semantic” level (Boyatzis, 1998), i.e. themes were initially organized to show patterns in semantic content. Inter-rater reliability, investigated through Cronbach’s alpha, was high (0.91). Results of the RTA related to the main explored themes are presented via BS’s historical evolution, detailed in Section 4.

We also derived some future trajectories for the BS field. These have been derived by looking for (1) topics that received scarce consideration by MD scholars and may deserve further attention due to their exciting implications, and (2) brand-new topics that emerged, within and outside MD, in recent years (2017–2024).

3.3 Historical approach

We decided to complement the RTA carried out on the 97 articles by implementing a historical analysis. Particularly, this decision is motivated by the fact that incorporating a historical perspective allowed us to trace the development of concepts, methodologies and key findings that have shaped the BS field. In fact, by examining the context in which past research has been conducted, we have been able to identify seminal works and shifts in research interests that have influenced the trajectory of what we now call BS – thus bringing a step forward in exploring this field of research compared to existing reviews.

The historical analysis gives readers a better understanding of the topic under analysis over time. For instance, Akinci and Sadler-Smith (2012) provided a historical review of intuition in management research, pointing out how insights and advancements from related fields (i.e. behavioral, biological and brain sciences) have enriched the conceptual, theoretical and methodological resources available to management scholars as well as “how lessons from the history of intuition research in management may illuminate the way forward (p. 104)”. Similarly, Linnenluecke (2017) studied resilience in business and management research, considering cultural contexts and organizational settings. In particular, the author suggests that the fragmentation of resilience research across various streams, as well as the future directions of this field of research, are linked to contextual events such as the financial crisis, concerns about climate change and many others (e.g. post-9/11 research shifted toward coping mechanisms and responses to external threats).

Following the examples mentioned above, we explored how MD shaped BS across three eras: (1) from the establishment of the journal until 1989. Indeed, it was around 1989 that strategic management gained recognition (Rabetino et al., 2021); (2) from 1990 to 2011. In this latter year, Powell et al. (2011) formally described and positioned the BS field; and (3) from 2012 to the present day. Further information is given in the following sections.

4. Results

4.1 Descriptive statistics

Given the article’s aim, this subsection supports readers’ understanding of the Journal’s contribution to the field of BS. Specifically, it helps readers become familiar with the main features of the BS articles published in MD and better comprehend the future trajectories we present later.

For example, the most prolific author in terms of contributions is George Panagiotou, who has three articles. Many authors have contributed, including two articles about BS published in MD: Steven H. Appelbaum, Joy Gandell, Juha-Antti Lamberg and Marcus Selart. Moreover, by examining the countries of affiliation of the first authors who published the 97 sample articles, it is possible to observe a predominance of authors from U.S.-based institutions (28; 29%) – this finds an explanation in North American scholars’ historical contribution to the rise of strategy research (Rabetino et al., 2021) – followed by British (12; 12%) and Canadian ones (7; 7%). However, in the last years, the research has been chiefly promoted by authors affiliated with Dutch (6; 6%), Italian (5; 5%), Indian (3; 3%) and Chinese (3; 3%) institutions – this trend could be attributed to the growth of MD readership. This latter assumption is supported by Figure 2, which shows a positive trend in the number of produced BS contributions in MD.

Among these articles, the most cited ones [2] are those authored by Tikkanen et al. (2005), Bolaños et al. (2005), Appelbaum et al. (2000) and Aspara et al. (2011). Furthermore, we underline the work advanced by Acciarini et al. (2021), which has already received 64 citations in less than two years; consequently, we hypothesize that this will be a highly cited BS work within MD and deserves to be reported. Yet, within the Emerald Literati Awards 2022, this latter contribution received the “Outstanding Article” recognition from the MD editorial board.

Concerning the theoretical approach, we aggregated the selected contributions according to the three schools of thought (i.e. reductionist, pluralist and contextualist) [3] proposed by Powell et al. (2011). As a result, we found that the preponderance of articles has adopted the contextualist standpoint (56; 58%), followed by the reductionist paradigm (29; 30%), and lastly, the pluralist school (12; 12%); thus, it is evident that the existing research on BS in MD is unbalanced toward the BS contextual level of analysis.

While in the first 20 years of the Journal, when authors began focusing on how managers make strategic decisions through discussions around early management theories (e.g. Rice, 1980), articles have adopted mainly a conceptual research design (46; 47%), nowadays scholars are essentially employing empirical methods (47; 48% – out of which there are 61% quantitative, 36% qualitative and 3% mixed methods), while only a tiny share is represented by reviews (4; 4%). Among the empirical studies, most of them have collected data through questionnaires (11%), semi-structured interviews (9%) and panel data (5%), then analyzed using either regression analyses (13%) or content analyses (9%).

4.2 Reflexive Thematic Analysis

Following Braun and Clarke (2019), the 97 sample articles have been systematically analyzed to identify the core behavioral elements in MD strategy articles.

As shown in Table 2, among the 39 produced codes, the most recurring are “models of decision-making” (N = 31, % = 19), “traps” (N = 12, % = 7), “mental models/cognitive maps/schemas” (N = 12, % = 7), “reference points” (N = 9, % = 5), “heuristics” (N = 9, % = 5) and “beliefs/personal value” (N = 8, % = 4.9). Produced codes were then grouped under eight broader themes according to the authors’ scientific knowledge and interpretation (researcher judgment is necessary to determine a theme; Braun and Clarke, 2019).

The eight themes that emerged are: “rationalization criteria,” “biases,” “thinking,” “perception,” “positive affective states,” “negative affective states,” “personality traits/attitudes” and “demographics.” These have been created by following the distinctions/conceptualizations advanced by previous scholars and defined as reported in Table 3.

5. The historical evolution of behavioral strategy in management decision

To provide a historical narrative of the evolution of BS, we ideally divided sample articles into three historical eras, in line with milestone events in the literature: (1) the end of the 80s, when strategic management was widely accepted as a scientific field, (2) 2011, when BS was described and positioned by Powell et al. (2011) and (3) the present day (see Figure 3).

First, the cluster Behavioral Strategy studies toward Strategic Management establishment (1967–1989) include contributions (n. 17) conceptualizing strategic management – the timespan was selected according to Rabetino et al. (2021), who stated that: “originating in the early 1960s, strategic management was widely accepted as a scientific field by the 1980s.” Second, Behavioral Strategy before Behavioral Strategy (1990–2011) contains studies (n. 37) well rooted in strategy (that we consider as “established” at the end of the 80s) with the integration of some behavioral aspects that were published before the positioning article of Powell et al. (2011). Third, Behavioral Strategy after Behavioral Strategy Positioning (2012–2024) considers contributions (n. 43) published after the cited theoretical positioning milestone (Powell et al., 2011). Indeed, despite the work of Powell et al. (2011) not depicting BS as a new idea, it signs the formal rise of BS. Such attribution has already been accepted, and authoritative contributions on the field have been integrated (e.g. Augier et al., 2018).

5.1 Behavioral strategy studies toward strategic management establishment (1967–1989)

BS contributions published in MD from 1967–1989 primarily focused on “models of decision-making” influenced by classical and neoclassical economic theories. According to Tyzack (1967), these models propose that executives should choose actions maximizing individual or collective utility to shape organizational behavior rationally. In this regard, Mueller (1968) argued for integrating management science findings to improve decision outcomes and advocated adopting a normative approach under specific conditions. Furthermore, Hertz (1972) discussed the discrepancy between good information input and low-quality decisions in corporate decision-making processes, highlighting the challenge of translating good information into effective decisions.

By the late “70s, the advent of Behavioral Decision Theory (BDT) – rooted in the concepts of bounded rationality (Simon, 1947) and cognitive limits (Tversky and Kahneman, 1974) – prompted a reassessment of ‘how executives make decisions.’ Rice (1980) contended that the rational model of classical and neoclassical economists fell short in addressing non-repetitive corporate problems; he sought to reconcile diverse theoretical approaches to decision-making with managerial practice. Drawing on BDT, emphasis was placed on recognizing executives” cognitive limitations as crucial for facilitating improved decision-making (Rice, 1980).

Expanding on the dominant coalition concept (Cyert and March, 1963), the focus shifted to collective models, aligning with contributions in decision-making models. Borwick (1978) identified five types of interaction strategies (i.e. acquiescence, defensive, compromise, conflict and problem-solving) among members, scrutinizing their human foundations and organizational outcomes (e.g. the acquiescence strategy relies on good relations, fostering social harmony and excellent firm performance, while the compromise strategy requires balancing interests for partial results).

Later, other studies explore decision-making models’ intricate social and psychological dynamics, which require continuous consideration and evaluation to reach strategic management objectives. In this regard, it is worthy to mention Gabor (1976), who affirmed that “decision-makers will not admit to the existence of emotional or intuitive factors in their decision calculus, preferring to justify their choice on narrow grounds associated with logic and science” (p. 280). Then, Parker (1980), studying group decision-making in business, emphasized that decision outcomes are shaped by a complex interplay of personal values, cognitive differences and group dynamics; thus, when group members harbor divergent values, conflicts may arise, priorities may clash, and consensus becomes challenging to achieve. In this regard, Ralston (1985) advocates the group participative decision-making model as the “management style of the future,” emphasizing decentralized group dynamics, extensive managerial autonomy within participatory frameworks, and the crucial role of maintaining equilibrium among individual behaviors and organizational cultures for fostering positive corporate outcomes (see also Mageean, 2984).

In this vein, O’Shaughnessy (1984) and White (1984) expanded the debate by pointing out the role of the environment in influencing expectations and decision-making processes. For instance, cultural values and norms have been highlighted as influential in shaping managers’ attitudes, perceptions and views on long-term planning. Similarly, managers’ education has been highlighted as an important factor influencing how managers make decisions (O’Shaughnessy, 1984). Drawing from social psychological theories of human behavior, Pate (1987) analyzed situations and managers’ decisions. Several sources of biases and perpetuating misunderstandings (i.e. traditions, opinions, rumors and advertising) were considered to prevent issues at a desirable corporate level. This last contribution represents an early approach toward building a multi-actor BS, leaving behind corporate decisions majorly based on apical figures’ intuitions.

However, in the same years, Hambrick and Mason (1984) renewed the scholarly interest in investigating top managers’ decision-making by forging the Upper Echelons Theory (UET). The UET, in a few words, considers organizations (and their performance) as “reflections of their top managers” (1984, p. 193; see Abatecola and Cristofaro, 2020 for an updated review). Namely, the board’s socio-demographic and psychological characteristics directly impact corporate performance via strategic management (e.g. the aging of the board influences the innovation rate via the introduction [or not] of a new product). Accordingly, some research on MD’s pages started developing this promising stream of study. For instance, Kiel and Blennerhasett (1984) focused on age. They examined the boards of 50 Australian companies, arguing that the existing presence of young board members was more able to face the complex challenges of a fast-developing environment.

5.2 Behavioral strategy before behavioral strategy (1990–2011)

In these thirty years, some scholars have focused on deepening the “rationalization criteria” theme; for instance, Feurer and Chaharbaghi (1995) broadened this discussion by moving toward a more human-centered approach, arguing that organizations’ success is linked to quantity and quality of skills available within the organizations. In this regard, a broad pool of scholars (e.g. Aspara et al., 2011) recognized the role played by the decision-maker’s abilities in selecting and handling the proper models of decision-making to maximize decision quality.

This shift has been fostered by the UET, which, being firmly anchored to BDT and its central concept of bounded rationality (Abatecola and Cristofaro, 2020), served to speed up a new stream of research around the “demographics” likely to affect decision-making processes. This has also been supported by Goll et al. (2008), who found a relationship between top management characteristics and specific choices (e.g. top management teams composed of younger managers emphasize strategy differentiation while those with greater education levels were more prone to service differentiation).

Contiguous with the previous theme, other scholars have investigated the “personality traits/attitudes,” which represent intangible dimensions but have a tremendous impact on decision-making. Precisely, Gallén (1997) linked personality traits with managers’ strategic decisions, proposing that specific strategies (i.e. the defender/reactor or analyzer/prospector) are associated with the personality decision-makers have (i.e. sensing-thinking/feeling or intuitive-thinking/feeling). Similarly, de Waal (2003) emphasized the role of 18 behavioral factors in allowing managers to implement and use performance management systems with positive repercussions on the quality of the corporate strategy, while Selart (2005) investigated the nexus between individuals’ increasing degrees of locus of control and the adoption specific strategic decision-making processes (i.e. participative, consultative and autocratic) finding a direct relationship among them.

Concerning attitudes, Mukherji and Hurtado (2001) found that decision-makers from different cultural contexts look at the environment differently; consequently, more is needed to consider how one organization looks at the environment and how their culturally diverse competitors do it. This converges with Shoham and Fiegenbaum (2002) about the strategic reference points – a construct used by organizations to evaluate all available strategic choices regarding risk – which vary according to exogenous factors (e.g. the cultural contexts).

Yet, another stream of scholars studied the role of thinking and perception factors in strategic conditioning decisions. Regarding the former, Bennett (1998) found that top managers could increase the quality of their choices by engaging in a synergistic exchange of knowledge to draw from a diversified set of insights. This is in line with Bolaños et al. (2005), who agreed that limiting individual and collective group information bias is crucial to increasing the awareness of strategic situations. About “perception,” Appelbaum et al. (2000) explored the impact that individual and collective perceptions have on creating and implementing specific strategic choices, i.e. the reaction of internal stakeholders to the possibility of pursuing a corporate merger. This link has found strong support in recent contributions (i.e. Wright, 2005), which concluded that the perception variable is central in influencing decisions in turbulent business landscapes. According to Tikkanen et al. (2005) and Panagiotou (2005), the way through which organizations perceive, organize and handle their business models is a direct consequence of the interrelationship between organizational culture and the cognitive belief system of the decision-makers since it generates the structures, routines and hierarchies behind strategic choices.

Nevertheless, the so-called thinking and perception abilities are unquestionably intertwined with the temporary emotional states affecting decision-makers (e.g. Cristofaro, 2019). In this regard, essential contributions had the “positive affective states” and “negative affective states” themes as core objects of analysis. For instance, Ericson (2010) argued that an in-depth analysis of both affective states can increase the comprehension of specific strategic choices.

Furthermore, it is essential to recall another stream of scholars who have studied the impact of “biases.” For instance, Tello et al. (2010) examined how both individual and collective heuristics seem to be equally spread among decision-makers from different institutions, while Chao (2011) investigated the most common traps emerging during the life cycle of organizations when engaged in strategic alliances and how they lead to dysfunctional behaviors.

5.3 Behavioral strategy after behavioral strategy positioning (2012–2024)

From 2012 to 2024, many scholars studied strategic decision-making processes by combining multiple perspectives (substantiated by our identified themes), informing the nascent BS field. For instance, by combining the “perception” and “rationalization criteria” literatures, Ridge et al. (2014) claimed that executives are often affected by strategic myopia: due to bounded rationality and learning dysfunctions, decision-makers consider a narrowed set of alternatives when trying to solve a strategic problem. Yet, Ramya and Baral (2021), by merging concepts belonging to the “thinking,” “perception” and “rationalization criteria” themes, proposed a framework aimed to enhance the accuracy of mental models via better sensemaking (i.e. by identifying the causes, constraints and predicting future outcomes related to specific events and actions) which, in turn, may be fostered by the introduction of nudging policies within the organization. As a direct consequence, decision-makers are pushed to search for relevant information that is likely to increase the strategic view of the company and, thus, the overall quality of the decisions made within the organizational boundaries.

Concerning the evolution of individuals’ characteristics over time, Haider and Mariotti (2016), taking from “perception” and “thinking,” have shown how the cognitive distortions about spatial and temporal cognition (i.e. the acquisition, organization and utilization of knowledge about the internal and external environments, and the set of cognitive functions that support the broad range of time experiences) vary across the stages of the decision-makers’ career. Similar findings have been disclosed by Kolbe et al. (2020), who, drawing from the “thinking” theme, pointed out that decision-makers usually rely on intuition in their early stage and then move toward an increasingly more rational approach in later ones. In this light, Olson et al. (2023) contributed to shed light on how “negative affective states” influence “thinking”; specifically, by integrating affect-cognitive theory and organizational learning theory (Cristofaro, 2020; Levitt and March, 1988), these authors argued that CEOs’ anger could drive error value recognition and learning.

Similarly, and by rooting the disclosed results on the premises stated in the UET, Midavaine et al. (2016), taking from the “demographics,” found that tenure diversity leads firms to be more risk-averse (e.g. investing less in research and development) – in line with Biscotti et al. (2018) who pointed out that executives’ turnover has a direct impact on their willingness to join open innovation projects, hence, sharing decision-making power among multiple parties. Moreover, Ali et al. (2023) and Kremer (2023) contributed to shed light on the role of “demographics.” Specifically, Ali et al. (2023) discussed how board diversity – measured in terms of gender, age and nationality, and cognitive diversity through education, expertise and tenure – helps reduce the likelihood of firms’ financial distress. Kremer (2023) showed how CEOs’ values influence their intention to downsize; in particular, CEOs’ conservation lowers the downsizing severity, while CEOs’ openness to change values increases it.

Indeed, considering the literature discussed above, it is possible to state that the personal characteristics of decision-makers affect the type/magnitude of cognitive distortions to which they are subjects (Mueller-Saegebrecht, 2024; Picone et al., 2024). In this regard, to achieve satisfactory levels of decision quality, decision-makers should aspire to have a less biased decision-making process possible; however, according to Coffeng et al. (2021), who took from the “thinking” dimension, executives are generally unaware of their biased strategic decision-making and thus unable to correct it. Relatedly, Abatecola (2014) theoretically advanced that CEOs/Top Management Teams (TMTs) initially adopt heuristics for solving business problems over the firm’s life cycle and that, in the presence of continuous positive feedback, adopted heuristics become the object of an overall self-reinforcing effect that determine the path of the organization. In this vein, Acciarini et al. (2021) highlighted the importance of feedback mechanisms to counteract biases and evaluate and adjust the smoothness of the overall decision-making process. See also Greco et al. (2021) who proposed corporate debiasing policies, and Frisk and Bannister (2017), who stressed the importance of data analytics for debiasing.

Lastly, the proximity and magnitude of recent global crises – i.e. the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic, the Russian invasion of Ukraine, and the related energy crisis – have dramatically shocked the business environment, representing a unicum since the end of the Second World War. In this regard, on the MD pages, a series of authors took the occasion to reassess behavioral responses to unlucky strategic events. Tabesh and Vera (2020) advanced a conceptual model that links improvisational decision-making and decision quality in crisis. Although comprehensive and intuitive decision-making is rooted in distinct information processing approaches, using “paradoxical thinking” – in which “contradictory yet interrelated elements that seem logical in isolation but absurd and irrational when appearing simultaneously” (Lewis, 2000, p. 760) – can be combined in unique ways when senior executives improvise decisions in crises.

6. Looking back and moving forward: future trajectories for behavioral strategy research

Herbert Simon’s (1947) groundbreaking concept of bounded rationality led to a significant shift in how scholars understand individual and group decision-making processes. This allowed the conceptualization, among others, of the Behavioral Theory of the Firm (Cyert and March 1963), the prospect theory (Kahneman and Tversky, 1979), heuristic and biases (Kahneman, 2011), the UET (Hambrick and Mason, 1984) and the nudge theory (Thaler, 2000). These milestone contributions have created fertile ground for BS, integrating cognitive and social psychology with strategic management to enrich theory and practice by considering realistic human behavior (Powell et al., 2011). This produced a wealth of literature on BS, which MD has successfully captured throughout its over half a century of activities.

Accordingly, this section details the recent progressions and changes in the academic interests in BS research published in MD and connects them with external sources to delineate the way forward. In this regard, Figure 4 shows the main themes discussed in each historical era of BS research (Section 5). As readers may note, there has been an unstable presence of themes over the years, a clear testimony of the field’s evolution from its embryonic stage to its current form. In fact, over time, a kind of “intellectual selection” has steered scholarly attention toward specific BS topics. Consequently, certain themes have waned (e.g. “rationalization criteria” between 1989 and 2011), while novel ones have ascended (e.g. “thinking,” “perception,” “positive/negative affective states” between 1989 and 2011), contributing to the evolution of the BS field.

Looking at what was produced from 1967 to 2024 in MD from a historical perspective, it is visible that BS research has been shaped by several significant changes, both in theoretical and methodological terms, thus co-evolving with the overall management thought. Similarly, by considering valuable BS research avenues that emerged from research inside and outside MD, we identified six emerging topics (see Table 4) that are likely to pave the way for the next era of BS research. These are: “positive heuristics,” “context-embedded mental processes,” “non-conventional thinking,” “cognitive evolutionary triggers,” “debiasing strategies” and “behavioral theories for new strategic challenges.” For each theme, we provide researchers with specific research questions (emerging from MD and non-MD research – the latter signed with an asterisk), the scientific paradigm (according to Powell et al., 2011) and the most fitting research methodologies.

Positive heuristics. Ecological rationality challenges the traditional notion of heuristics as suboptimal shortcuts (Kahneman and Tversky, 1979) and emphasizes their positive role in problem-solving and predictions (Todd and Gigerenzer, 2012). Recent research in MD explores the benefits of specific heuristics, under some conditions, in business. For example, Atanasiu (2021) presents a managerial heuristic model, while Cavarretta (2021) underscores the role of social calculations in developing heuristics for pragmatic action theories. However, caution is needed in generalizing these positive effects. For instance, business-specific scenarios, like individual heuristics based on organizational identities (Greco et al., 2021), necessitate field studies and multivariate statistics for a comprehensive assessment of the effects of corporate strategy change (see also Luan et al., 2019). In this vein, MD literature emphasizes the importance of investigating the self-confidence heuristic in overcoming a firm’s status quo (Galavotti et al., 2021). Thus, contextualist approaches, such as ethnographic methodologies (e.g. Elbanna and Newman, 2022), are crucial for evaluating the effect of top management self-confidence on corporate strategic decisions. In line with Guercini and Lechner (2021), future research should focus on integrating heuristics and AI algorithms for improved business decisions (see also Shrestha et al., 2019). Moreover, decision-making simulations, incorporating technologies like eye-tracking (Wang et al., 2014) or Electroencephalogram (EEG) experiments (Li et al., 2022), can synergize human intuition and technology for higher strategic decision quality (see also Bader and Kaiser, 2019). All in all, this is also in line with the recent call for future research by Neely et al. (2020). Specifically, these authors, by commenting on the seminal work by Hambrick and Mason (1984), recommended integrating knowledge from diverse fields, utilizing recent methodological advancements, and cautiously employing biotechnologies such as EEGs and wearable trackers to enhance the understanding of the biological basis of executives’ cognition.

Context-Embedded Mental Processes. Recently, scholars in MD have investigated the interplay between individuals’ cognitive capabilities and their social, cultural and physical contexts in decision-making. Notably, Ahmadzadeh et al. (2022) explored collective stupidity’s antecedents and consequences, emphasizing its impact on strategic decision-making outcomes (see also Alvesson and Spicer, 2012), while Yu et al. (2022) delved into how collective reputation cognition shapes innovation behavior, guiding firms toward high innovation performance. However, a gap persists in scaling the individual behavior to the collective behavior group level. Hence, we propose a multi-paradigmatic approach using field studies and multivariate statistics to explore how business context characteristics facilitate scaling cognitive capacities to collective behaviors and vice versa (Akinci and Sadler-Smith, 2019; Joseph and Gaba, 2020). This future direction also finds support beyond MD. Bromiley and Rau (2016) advocate for future research in BS to effectively categorize context-embedded mental processes, stressing the importance of distinguishing between cognitive and social influences on the strategy process to prevent overlapping variables and enable investigation of their mutual effects. More recently, these scholars, stemming from the prospect theory (Kahneman and Tversky, 1979), emphasized the necessity of comprehending individual and organizational contexts to grasp decision-making intricacies affected by the curvature of the value function. Specifically, they highlight the necessity to bridge theoretical underpinnings with practical insights to predict and explicate real-world decisions within organizational settings (Bromiley and Rau, 2016).

Non-conventional thinking. In today’s complex business environment, decision-makers need to imagine new possibilities and solve complex problems using a combination of intuition and rationality (Dane and Pratt, 2007; Hodgkinson and Healey, 2011; Helfat and Peteraf, 2015). In MD, scholars have dedicated efforts to enhance the combined understanding of these two. Okoli and Watt (2018) explored how experienced crisis managers employ intuitive and analytical strategies, called “non-conventional thinking,” during complex incidents. However, future research should investigate the conditions under which non-conventional thinking is beneficial or detrimental to strategic management (see also Miller and Ireland, 2005; Calabretta et al., 2017). To address these questions, we encourage using technologies to explore the balance between decision-making processes requiring high cognitive effort and those dominated by intuition (Luoma and Martela, 2021; Yu et al., 2023). Data collected through technological applications can be analyzed using quantitative models to determine when non-conventional thinking should be promoted or discouraged.

Cognitive evolutionary triggers. While research in strategic management has explored the cognitive abilities guiding managers’ decision-making in various contexts, scant attention has been given to the impact of external determinants on managerial cognitive abilities in BS (Acciarini et al., 2021). Contributions in MD, like Norris et al. (2020), examined the pandemic as a trigger for education administrators’ cognitive shortcuts, emphasizing the influence of external events on decision-making. Similarly, Abatecola (2014) proposed a model discussing self-reinforcing processes in managerial decision-making, drawing from the biological philosophy of organizational change. In this regard, recent research opportunities arise from significant external changes (e.g. the pandemic and climate crisis) that impact human emotional and cognitive aspects (see also Combe and Carrington, 2015; Kim et al., 2021; Miocevic, 2021). This assumption is also backed by scholars such as Argote and Greve (2007) and Gavetti et al. (2012), who, stemming from Cyert and March (1963), argued that managers’ cognitive representations play a significant role in shaping how organizations search for opportunities, make decisions and adapt to changes in their environment. Thus, evaluating the effects of these events on managers’ cognitive mechanisms through mathematical and computational modeling using data collected before and after triggers would be intriguing. Even outside MD, there is interest in understanding similarities among cognitive evolutionary triggers and categorizing them based on specific criteria (Hesselbarth et al., 2023). This exploration could empower organizations to exert more control over cognitive evolutionary triggers, potentially enabling the conscious triggering of controlled shocks to enhance managers’ cognitive abilities.

Debiasing strategies. Due to their dual significance, debiasing strategies have been thoroughly investigated in behavioral research (e.g. Cantarelli et al., 2020; Hristov et al., 2022; Lovallo et al., 2023). Primarily, strategists are encouraged to apply debiasing strategies to bolster their cognitive processes for enhancing firm planning. Moreover, debiasing strategies can improve individuals’ cognitive processes across various corporate departments when integrated into behavioral plans. Nevertheless, further research in this area is needed despite the attention received within the MD community (Anwar et al., 2021; Yamini, 2021). Indeed, we recommend reconsidering methodologies, emphasizing empirical research to generate accurate primary data on human emotional, cognitive and social factors that can verify the effectiveness of debiasing strategies, also considering the massive presence of human–AI interaction for corporate strategic decision-making (see also Daugherty and Wilson, 2018; Raisch and Krakowski, 2021).

Behavioral theories for new strategic challenges. Behavioral theories offer valuable insights to firms for addressing new strategic challenges (e.g. diversity inclusion, digital transformation and sustainable development), as demonstrated by most recent research predominantly developed out of MD. For instance, Waldman and Sparr (2023), drawing on Positive Organizational Behavior theory, interpret the effects of different strategies (i.e. “woke” and “integrative”) to face the matter of diversity inclusion within organizations successfully. Moreover, Virmani et al. (2023) employ Behavioral Reasoning Theory to analyze managers’ behavioral patterns that push toward Industry 4.0 adoption, thus providing insights for various stakeholders and encouraging further exploration across different business settings. In this vein, we want to encourage future BS research, especially within MD, to move on a double track: using behavioral theories to manage upcoming corporate strategic challenges and adapting such theories under the blows of the same recent changes.

7. Conclusions

This work mainly contributes to tracing the BS field’s historical evolution and provides insight into the challenges and opportunities in the research field. In doing that, it fills a gap in the study of the evolution of management thought and celebrates the Journal’s “Over-half a century of Management Decision” informal section. In particular, we offer a historical overview of consolidated (e.g. positive heuristics) and underexplored (i.e. debiasing strategies) areas, helping future BS researchers, especially in MD, avoid redundant research. Provided future research trajectories are built on sample articles and selected international literature outside MD, thus providing a research plan. This article also brings practical implications. Practitioners are suggested to invest in training programs that focus on understanding drivers influencing strategic decision-making processes, implementing regular behavioral audits, promoting a culture of self-awareness within their teams and leveraging decision-making tools designed to minimize biases (e.g. the 12-question checklist proposed by Kahneman et al., 2011). These practical insights can mitigate biases, enhance team collaboration and lead to a more objective and effective strategic decision-making process.

In conclusion, this article is not exempt from limitations. The main focus is on management decisions to investigate BS’s historical evolution. This overlooks key contributions published in other outlets and may be influenced by MD’s editorial preferences. A greater understanding requires broader journal coverage to mitigate biases and enhance field appraisal; thus, a more comprehensive review of the BS field and a formal organizing framework of BS research are still needed. We believe that the future of strategic management research is in the study of executives’ behavior. Otherwise, we will always be far from understanding how organizations act strategically.

Figures

Flowchart of papers’ collection strategy

Figure 1

Flowchart of papers’ collection strategy

Annual scientific production of BS articles in MD

Figure 2

Annual scientific production of BS articles in MD

No. of papers on BS published in Management Decision between 1967 and 2024 according to their historical clusterization

Figure 3

No. of papers on BS published in Management Decision between 1967 and 2024 according to their historical clusterization

The progression and changes in the academic interests toward BS research over time

Figure 4

The progression and changes in the academic interests toward BS research over time

Sub-sample of selected papers

#Author(s) a)Year b)Type of paper c)Data collection d)Data analysis e)Main results f)Scientific paradigm g)Code h)
1Tyzack, JE.V.1967ConceptualIn this article, the author finds that directors’ personal characteristics (e.g. demographic ones) are likely to alter the way in which they make strategic decisions within the organizationsContextualistModels of decision-making
2Appelbaum, S.H., Gandell, J., Yortis, H., Proper, S., and Jobin, F.2000ConceptualIn this paper, it has been disclosed that organizational factors such as communications, corporate culture, change, and stress have a major role in influencing pre-merger, during the merger and post-merger stagesContextualistMemory
3Bolaños, R., Fontela, E., Nenclares, A., and Pastor, P.2005Empirical qualitativeSimulation with a not specified sample of studentsInterpretive structural modelingThe authors have found major differences in priority orderings of the different managerial roles which will make it more difficult to take a decision within organizationsPluralistReference points
4Aspara, J., Lamberg, J.A., Laukia, A., and Tikkanen, H.2011Empirical qualitativeArchival material (i.e. board meeting protocols and memos, correspondence between corporate headquarters and business units, circular letters, strategic planning documents and market analyses) and 14 interviews among former Nokia executives and other experts from the telecommunication industryHistorical analysisThe authors have demonstrated the central role of business units in feeding strategic alternatives and capabilities to the corporate-level transformation processContextualistBundles of rules
5Al-Shammari, M.A., Banerjee, S.N., Al-Shammari, H., and Doty, H.2023Empirical quantitative9,348 firm-year observations from the Kinder, Lydenberg and Domini (KLD) CSR databaseOLS regressionThis study aims to investigate how the association between corporate social responsibility (CSR) and firm performance, documented in prior research, is affected by the joint effects of managerial ability and attributes of the firm’s governance structureReductionistModels of decision-making

Source(s): Own elaboration

Number and frequency of BS codes and themes

Theme(s)Code(s)N%
Rationalization criteriaBundles of rules411
Models of decision-making3189
Total35100
BiasesHeuristics940
Hubris14
Traps1255
Total22100
ThinkingCreativity27
Intuition621
Mental models/cognitive maps/schemas1241
Reference points931
Total29100
PerceptionAttention421
Beliefs/personal values842
Memory211
Sensemaking526
Total19100
Positive affective statesPositive emotions150
Positive mood150
Total2100
Negative affective statesNegative emotions675
Negative mood225
Total8100
Personality traits/attitudesAdaptability14
Autocratic14
Fanaticism14
Feeling-type14
Locus of control14
Narcissism14
Openness14
Participative14
People-oriented14
Personal identity313
Proactivity14
Risk-taking attitude521
Self-enhancement14
Self-transcendence14
Sensing-type14
Task-oriented14
Trusting14
Total23100
DemographicsAge519
Education623
Functional background623
Gender311
Tenure623
Total26100
TOTAL164100

Source(s): Own elaboration

BS themes’ definitions

Theme(s)Definition(s)
Rationalization criteriaThe practical boundaries within which decision-makers make tailored decisions for their organizations (e.g. Tyzack, 1967; Aspara et al., 2011)
BiasesCognitive distortions (i.e. traps and heuristics) that affect either negatively, positively, or alternately, the thinking mechanisms to make the best decision (e.g. Acciarini et al., 2021)
ThinkingMechanisms (e.g. Mental models/cognitive maps/schemas) used by decision-makers during the decision-making process (e.g. Bennett, 1998)
PerceptionElements that help decision-makers to translate stimuli into the antecedents of a cognitive process (e.g. sensemaking, beliefs and attention) (e.g. Ericson, 2010)
Positive affective statesPositive short-term (i.e. emotions) and long-term (moods) emotional states are expected to enhance the quality of decision-making processes (e.g. Pate, 1987)
Negative affective statesNegative short-term (i.e. emotions) and long-term (moods) emotional states are expected to lower the quality of decision-making processes (e.g. Kouamé et al., 2015)
Personality traits/attitudesThe relatively stable, consistent and enduring internal characteristics that are inferred from patterns of behaviors, attitudes, feelings and habits in the individuals (e.g. Abatecola, 2014)
DemographicsSocio-demographic variables (e.g. gender, age, level of education and tenure of office) likely to influence the decision-making process and thus its outputs (e.g. Midavaine et al., 2016)

BS trajectories – from MD and non-MD* research

Theme(s)Definition(s)Future avenuesPotential research questionsSuggested methodologies
Positive heuristicsThe view that simple rules can be more effective or the only feasible option in given situations for making appropriate decisions, considering their effectiveness beyond ideological prejudicesNew cutting-edge theories, methodologies and tools may enhance the evaluation of heuristics’ final effects on differentiated business contexts as well as offer new ways of integrating heuristics and disruptive solutions for achieving outstanding decision qualityWhich common business context characteristics trigger positive heuristics acquisition?Decision experiments with control groups
What is the definitive effect of individual heuristics, generated by different organizational identities, on corporate strategy change?Field studies, multivariate statistics
Does managers’ self-confidence help to overcome a firm’s status quo and catch business opportunities in a changing environment?Interpretive histories, ethnography, hermeneutics
Which Artificial Intelligence tools enhance the positive effect on decision quality of positive heuristics?Hypothesis testing, decision experiments,
simulation, neural
methods
Context-embedded mental processesIndividuals’ cognitive capacities reflect the social, cultural and physical contexts in which they are asked to make decisionsFostering the adoption of a multi-paradigmatic approach to comprehensively investigate the integration of both individual and collective psychology in organizationsWhich business context characteristics favor scaling-up from certain individual cognitive capacities to collective behaviors and vice versa?Field studies, multivariate statistics
How to categorize cognitive and social influences on the strategy process?*Interpretive histories, ethnography, grounded
theorizing
hermeneutics, textual
analysis, discourse
analysis, semiotics
cases
Non-conventional thinkingThe ability of decision-makers to imagine new possibilities and solve complex problems by combining ideas using both intuition and rationality and frame this interplay as a sustainable and virtuous tensionAdopting innovative tools that can allow BS scholars to untangle the executive’s black boxWhich are the cases where rational or intuitive-driven decision-making processes are employed?Neural methods
What are the cases where a rational or intuitive-driven decision-making process is more effective?Hypothesis testing, decision experiments,
simulation
mathematical and computational
modeling, neural
methods
Cognitive evolutionary triggersSeries of external stimuli (e.g. dramatic and unpredictable business situations) that push the cognitive abilities of decision-makers to evolve as a direct consequence of such situationsEvaluating the impact of recent and unpredictable global challenges, working as exogenous triggers, on cognitive mechanisms of strategistsWhat are the impacts of the pandemic on managers’ cognitive abilities in strategic decision-making processes?Mathematical and computational
modeling
What are the impacts of climate change on managers’ cognitive abilities in strategic decision-making processes?Mathematical and computational
modeling
What are the commonalities among cognitive evolutionary triggers?Mathematical and computational
modeling
How do categorize cognitive evolutionary triggers impacting the strategic decision-making process?*Interpretive histories, ethnography, grounded
theorizing
hermeneutics, textual
analysis, discourse
analysis, semiotics
cases
Debiasing strategiesCorporate practices to reduce the negative effect of specific cognitive biases on corporate strategic decision-making processIdentifying debiasing strategies and evaluating their effectivenessWhat are effective debiasing strategies to improve strategic decision-making quality?*Hypothesis testing, decision experiments, simulation, mathematical and computational modeling, neural methods
How to break organizational barriers toward an effective implementation of debiasing strategies?*Field studies, event studies, multivariate
statistics, cases, mixed
methods
Behavioral theories for new strategic challengesTheories explaining human behaviors in times of external changes that generate strategic challenges for firmsExploiting and adapting behavioral theories to manage recent strategic challengesWhich are the behavioral theories most suitable for firms to address recent strategic challenges?*Hypothesis testing, decision experiments, simulation, mathematical and computational modeling, neural methods
How to adapt behavioral theories to help firms address recent strategic challenges?*Hypothesis testing, decision experiments, simulation, mathematical and computational modeling, neural methods

Source(s): Own elaboration

Notes

1.

This term was originally coined by Lovallo and Sibony (2010).

2.

Articles that have at least 100 citations according to Scopus (as of July 15th, 2023).

3.

The reductionist school is grounded on positivist, realist, and objectivist philosophies; its research favors quantitative hypothesis testing using mathematical modeling, simulation and laboratory decision experiments. The pluralist school believes in positivist, nominalist, pragmatist or evolutionary philosophies; its research is less concerned with individual decision-making than with the overall decision environment of the firm. The contextualist school relies on phenomenological, constructivist and critical philosophies, and its research emphasizes the primacy of context (Powell et al., 2011).

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found online.

References (*points out articles in the review sample)

*Abatecola, G. (2014), “Untangling self-reinforcing processes in managerial decision making. Co-evolving heuristics?”, Management Decision, Vol. 52 No. 5, pp. 934-949, doi: 10.1108/md-10-2013-0543.

Abatecola, G. and Cristofaro, M. (2020), “Hambrick and Mason's ‘upper Echelons theory’: evolution and open avenues”, Journal of Management History, Vol. 26 No. 1, pp. 116-136, doi: 10.1108/jmh-02-2018-0016.

*Acciarini, C., Brunetta, F. and Boccardelli, P. (2021), “Cognitive biases and decision-making strategies in times of change: a systematic literature review”, Management Decision, Vol. 59 No. 3, pp. 638-652, doi: 10.1108/md-07-2019-1006.

*Ahmadzadeh, S., Safari, A. and Teimouri, H. (2022), “Collective stupidity: influences on decision-making in knowledge-based companies”, Management Decision, Vol. 60 No. 5, pp. 1257-1295, doi: 10.1108/md-10-2020-1380.

Akinci, C. and Sadler-Smith, E. (2012), “Intuition in management research: a historical review”, International Journal of Management Reviews, Vol. 14 No. 1, pp. 104-122, doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2370.2011.00313.x.

Akinci, C. and Sadler‐Smith, E. (2019), “Collective intuition: implications for improved decision making and organizational learning”, British Journal of Management, Vol. 30 No. 3, pp. 558-577, doi: 10.1111/1467-8551.12269.

Al-Shammari, M.A., Banerjee, S.N., Al-Shammari, H. and Doty, H. (2023), “The interplay of CEO ability and governance robustness on the performance effects of corporate social responsibility”, Management Decision, Vol. 61 No. 7, pp. 1932-1965, doi: 10.1108/md-07-2022-0957.

*Ali, S., ur Rehman, R., Aslam, S., Khan, I. and Murtaza, G. (2023), “Does board diversity reduce the likelihood of financial distress in the presence of a powerful Chinese CEO?”, Management Decision, Vol. 61 No. 6, pp. 1798-1815, doi: 10.1108/md-01-2022-0007.

Alvesson, M. and Spicer, A. (2012), “A Stupidity‐Based theory of organizations”, Journal of Management Studies, Vol. 49 No. 7, pp. 1194-1220, doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6486.2012.01072.x.

Anwar, J., Bibi, A. and Ahmad, N. (2021), “Behavioral strategy: mapping the trends, sources and intellectual evolution”, Journal of Strategy and Management, Vol. 15 No. 1, pp. 140-168, doi: 10.1108/jsma-01-2021-0002.

*Appelbaum, S.H., Gandell, J., Yortis, H., Proper, S. and Jobin, F. (2000), “Anatomy of a merger: behavior of organizational factors and processes throughout the pre‐ during‐ post‐stages (part I)”, Management Decision, Vol. 38 No. 9, pp. 649-662, doi: 10.1108/00251740010357267.

Argote, L. and Greve, H.R. (2007), “A behavioral theory of the firm – 40 years and counting: introduction and impact”, Organization Science, Vol. 18 No. 3, pp. 337-349, doi: 10.1287/orsc.1070.0280.

*Aspara, J., Lamberg, J.A., Laukia, A. and Tikkanen, H. (2011), “Strategic management of business model transformation: lessons from Nokia”, Management Decision, Vol. 49 No. 4, pp. 622-647, doi: 10.1108/00251741111126521.

*Atanasiu, R. (2021), “The lifecycle of heuristics as managerial proverbs”, Management Decision, Vol. 59 No. 7, pp. 1617-1641, doi: 10.1108/md-08-2019-1025.

Augier, M., Fang, C. and Rindova, V. (Eds) (2018), Behavioral Strategy in Perspective, Emerald Group Publishing, Leeds.

Bader, V. and Kaiser, S. (2019), “Algorithmic decision-making? The user interface and its role for human involvement in decisions supported by artificial intelligence”, Organization, Vol. 26 No. 5, pp. 655-672, doi: 10.1177/1350508419855714.

*Bennett, R.H. (1998), “The importance of tacit knowledge in strategic deliberations and decisions”, Management Decision, Vol. 36 No. 9, pp. 589-597, doi: 10.1108/00251749810239478.

*Biscotti, A.M., Mafrolla, E., Giudice, M.D. and D'Amico, E. (2018), “CEO turnover and the new leader propensity to open innovation: Agency-resource dependence view and social identity perspective”, Management Decision, Vol. 56 No. 6, pp. 1348-1364, doi: 10.1108/md-04-2017-0392.

*Bolaños, R., Fontela, E., Nenclares, A. and Pastor, P. (2005), “Using interpretive structural modelling in strategic decision‐making groups”, Management Decision, Vol. 43 No. 6, pp. 877-895, doi: 10.1108/00251740510603619.

Borchardt, W., Kamzabek, T. and Lovallo, D. (2022), “Behavioral strategy in the wild”, Management Research Review, Vol. 45 No. 9, pp. 1185-1204, doi: 10.1108/mrr-12-2021-0876.

*Borwick, I. (1978), “Management and interaction strategy”, Management Decision, Vol. 16 No. 6, pp. 350-361, doi: 10.1108/eb001535.

Boyatzis, R.E. (1998), Transforming Qualitative Information: Thematic Analysis and Code Development, SAGE, Thousand Oaks, CA.

Braun, V. and Clarke, V. (2019), “Reflecting on reflexive thematic analysis”, Qualitative Research in Sport, Exercise and Health, Vol. 11 No. 4, pp. 589-597, doi: 10.1080/2159676x.2019.1628806.

Bromiley, P. and Rau, D. (2016), “Social, behavioral, and cognitive influences on upper echelons during strategy process: a literature review”, Journal of Management, Vol. 42 No. 1, pp. 174-202, doi: 10.1177/0149206315617240.

Calabretta, G., Gemser, G. and Wijnberg, N.M. (2017), “The interplay between intuition and rationality in strategic decision making: a paradox perspective”, Organization Studies, Vol. 38 Nos 3-4, pp. 365-401, doi: 10.1177/0170840616655483.

Cantarelli, P., Belle, N. and Belardinelli, P. (2020), “Behavioral public HR: experimental evidence on cognitive biases and debiasing interventions”, Review of Public Personnel Administration, Vol. 40 No. 1, pp. 56-81, doi: 10.1177/0734371x18778090.

Cavarretta, F.L. (2021), “On the hard problem of selecting bundles of rules: a conceptual exploration of heuristic emergence process”, Management Decision, Vol. 59 No. 7, pp. 1598-1616.

*Chao, Y. (2011), “Decision‐making biases in the alliance life cycle: implications for alliance failure”, Management Decision, Vol. 49 No. 3, pp. 350-364, doi: 10.1108/00251741111120743.

*Coffeng, T., Van Steenbergen, E.F., De Vries, F. and Ellemers, N. (2021), “Quality of group decisions by board members: a hidden-profile experiment”, Management Decision, Vol. 59 No. 13, pp. 38-55, doi: 10.1108/md-07-2020-0893.

Combe, I.A. and Carrington, D.J. (2015), “Leaders' sensemaking under crises: emerging cognitive consensus over time within management teams”, The Leadership Quarterly, Vol. 26 No. 3, pp. 307-322, doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2015.02.002.

Cristofaro, M. (2017), “Herbert Simon's bounded rationality: its evolution in management and cross-feritilizing contribution”, Journal of Management History, Vol. 23 No. 2, pp. 170-190, doi: 10.1108/jmh-11-2016-0060.

Cristofaro, M. (2019), “The role of affect in management decisions: a systematic review”, European Management Journal, Vol. 37 No. 1, pp. 6-17, doi: 10.1016/j.emj.2018.12.002.

Cristofaro, M. (2020), “‘I feel and think, therefore I am’: an affect-cognitive theory of management decisions”, European Management Journal, Vol. 38 No. 2, pp. 344-355, doi: 10.1016/j.emj.2019.09.003.

Cristofaro, M., Giannetti, F. and Abatecola, G. (2023), “The initial survival of the Unicorns: a behavioral perspective of Snapchat”, Journal of Management History, Vol. 29 No. 4, pp. 456-480, doi: 10.1108/jmh-11-2022-0066.

Cyert, R.M. and March, J. (1963), A Behavioral Theory of the Firm, Prentice Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ.

Dane, E. and Pratt, M.G. (2007), “Exploring intuition and its role in managerial decision making”, Academy of Management Review, Vol. 32 No. 1, pp. 33-54, doi: 10.5465/amr.2007.23463682.

Daugherty, P.R. and Wilson, H.J. (2018), Human+ Machine: Reimagining Work in the Age of AI, Harvard Business Press, Boston, MA.

Davidson, M.J. and Cooper, C.L. (1987), “The pressures on women managers”, Management Decision, Vol. 25 No. 4, pp. 57-63, doi: 10.1108/eb001464.

*de Waal, A.A. (2003), “Behavioral factors important for the successful implementation and use of performance management systems”, Management Decision, Vol. 41 No. 8, pp. 688-697, doi: 10.1108/00251740310496206.

Edmondson, A.C. and McManus, S.E. (2007), “Methodological fit in management field research”, Academy of Management Review, Vol. 32 No. 4, pp. 1246-1264, doi: 10.5465/amr.2007.26586086.

Elbanna, A. and Newman, M. (2022), “The bright side and the dark side of top management support in Digital Transformation – a hermeneutical reading”, Technological Forecasting and Social Change, Vol. 175, 121411, doi: 10.1016/j.techfore.2021.121411.

Elbanna, S., Kapoutsis, I. and Mellahi, K. (2017), “Creativity and propitiousness in strategic decision making: the role of positive politics and macro-economic uncertainty”, Management Decision, Vol. 55 No. 10, pp. 2218-2236, doi: 10.1108/md-02-2017-0113.

*Ericson, M. (2010), “Towards a sensed decision-making approach: from déjà vu to vu jàdé”, Management Decision, Vol. 48 No. 1, pp. 132-155, doi: 10.1108/00251741011014490.

*Feurer, R. and Chaharbaghi, K. (1995), “Strategy development: past, present and future”, Management Decision, Vol. 33 No. 6, pp. 11-21, doi: 10.1108/00251749510087614.

Foss, N.J. (2020), “Behavioral strategy and the COVID-19 disruption”, Journal of Management, Vol. 46 No. 8, pp. 1322-1329, doi: 10.1177/0149206320945015.

*Frisk, J.E. and Bannister, F. (2017), “Improving the use of analytics and big data by changing the decision-making culture: a design approach”, Management Decision, Vol. 55 No. 10, pp. 2074-2088, doi: 10.1108/md-07-2016-0460.

Gabor, P. (1976), “Management theory and rational decision making”, Management Decision, Vol. 14 No. 5, pp. 274-281.

Galavotti, I., Lippi, A. and Cerrato, D. (2021), “The representativeness heuristic at work in decision-making: building blocks and individual cognitive and behavioral factors”, Management Decision, Vol. 59 No. 7, pp. 1664-1683.

*Gallén, T. (1997), “The cognitive style and strategic decisions of managers”, Management Decision, Vol. 35 No. 7, pp. 541-551, doi: 10.1108/00251749710170501.

Gavetti, G., Greve, H.R., Levinthal, D.A. and Ocasio, W. (2012), “The behavioral theory of the firm: assessment and prospects”, Academy of Management Annals, Vol. 6 No. 1, pp. 1-40.

*Goll, I., Brown Johnson, N. and Rasheed, A.A. (2008), “Top management team demographic characteristics, business strategy, and firm performance in the US airline industry: the role of managerial discretion”, Management Decision, Vol. 46 No. 2, pp. 201-222, doi: 10.1108/00251740810854122.

Gordon, A.V., Ramic, M., Rohrbeck, R. and Spaniol, M.J. (2020), “50 Years of corporate and organizational foresight: looking back and going forward”, Technological Forecasting and Social Change, Vol. 154, 119966, doi: 10.1016/j.techfore.2020.119966.

*Greco, A., Long, T. and de Jong, G. (2021), “Identity reflexivity: a framework of heuristics for strategy change in hybrid organizations”, Management Decision, Vol. 59 No. 7, pp. 1684-1705, doi: 10.1108/md-10-2019-1369.

Guercini, S. and Lechner, C. (2021), “Guest editorial: new challenges for business actors and positive heuristics”, Management Decision, Vol. 59 No. 7, pp. 1585-1597, doi: 10.1108/md-07-2021-118.

*Haider, S. and Mariotti, F. (2016), “Unfolding critical events and strategic decisions: the role of spatial and temporal cognition”, Management Decision, Vol. 54 No. 7, pp. 1813-1842, doi: 10.1108/md-11-2015-0521.

Hambrick, D.C. and Mason, P.A. (1984), “Upper echelons: the organization as a reflection of its top managers”, Academy of Management Review, Vol. 9 No. 2, pp. 193-206, doi: 10.2307/258434.

Helfat, C.E. and Peteraf, M.A. (2015), “Managerial cognitive capabilities and the microfoundations of dynamic capabilities”, Strategic Management Journal, Vol. 36 No. 6, pp. 831-850, doi: 10.1002/smj.2247.

*Hertz, D. (1972), “Management science and the chief executive”, Management Decision, Vol. 10 No. 3, pp. 253-261, doi: 10.1108/eb001000.

Hesselbarth, I., Alnoor, A. and Tiberius, V. (2023), “Behavioral strategy: a systematic literature review and research framework”, Management Decision, Vol. 61 No. 9, pp. 2740-2756, doi: 10.1108/md-09-2021-1274.

Hodgkinson, G.P. and Healey, M.P. (2011), “Psychological foundations of dynamic capabilities: reflexion and reflection in strategic management”, Strategic Management Journal, Vol. 32 No. 13, pp. 1500-1516, doi: 10.1002/smj.964.

Hristov, I., Camilli, R. and Mechelli, A. (2022), “Cognitive biases in implementing a performance management system: behavioral strategy for supporting managers' decision-making processes”, Management Research Review, Vol. 45 No. 9, pp. 1110-1136, doi: 10.1108/mrr-11-2021-0777.

Joseph, J. and Gaba, V. (2020), “Organizational structure, information processing, and decision-making: a retrospective and road map for research”, Academy of Management Annals, Vol. 14 No. 1, pp. 267-302, doi: 10.5465/annals.2017.0103.

Kahneman, D. (2011), Thinking, Fast and Slow, Farrar, Straus and Giroux, New York City, NY.

Kahneman, D. and Tversky, A. (1979), “Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk”, Econometrica, Vol. 47 No. 2, pp. 263-291, doi: 10.2307/1914185.

Kahneman, D., Lovallo, D. and Sibony, O. (2011), “Before you make that big decision”, Harvard Business Review, Vol. 89 No. 6, pp. 50-60.

*Kiel, G. and Blennerhasett, P. (1984), “The board of directors in large Australian companies”, Management Decision, Vol. 22 No. 1, pp. 40-44, doi: 10.1108/eb001338.

Kim, J., Lee, H.W., Gao, H. and Johnson, R.E. (2021), “When CEOs are all about themselves: perceived CEO narcissism and middle managers' workplace behaviors amid the COVID-19 pandemic”, Journal of Applied Psychology, Vol. 106 No. 9, pp. 1283-1298, doi: 10.1037/apl0000965.

*Kolbe, L.M., Bossink, B. and de Man, A.P. (2020), “Contingent use of rational, intuitive and political decision-making in R&D”, Management Decision, Vol. 58 No. 6, pp. 997-1020, doi: 10.1108/md-02-2019-0261.

Kouamé, S., Oliver, D. and Poisson-de-Haro, S. (2015), “Can emotional differences be a strenght? Affective diversity and managerial decision performance”, Management Decision, Vol. 53 No. 8, pp. 1662-1676.

*Kremer, R. (2023), “Opening up their values: how CEOs conservation and openness to change impact downsizing decisions”, Management Decision, Vol. 61 No. 9, pp. 2512-2540, doi: 10.1108/md-01-2022-0122.

Levitt, B. and March, J.G. (1988), “Organizational learning”, Annual Review of Sociology, Vol. 14 No. 1, pp. 319-338, doi: 10.1146/annurev.so.14.080188.001535.

Lewis, M.W. (2000), “Exploring paradox: toward a more comprehensive guide”, Academy of Management Review, Vol. 25 No. 4, pp. 760-776, doi: 10.2307/259204.

Li, S., Lyu, T., Chen, M. and Zhang, P. (2022), “The prospects of using EEG in tourism and hospitality research”, Journal of Hospitality and Tourism Research, Vol. 46 No. 1, pp. 189-211, doi: 10.1177/1096348021996439.

Linnenluecke, M.K. (2017), “Resilience in business and management research: a review of influential publications and a research agenda”, International Journal of Management Reviews, Vol. 19 No. 1, pp. 4-30, doi: 10.1111/ijmr.12076.

Lovallo, D. and Sibony, O. (2010), “The case for behavioral strategy”, McKinsey Quarterly, No. 2, pp. 30-40.

Lovallo, D. and Sibony, O. (2018), “Broadening the frame: how behavioral strategy redefines strategic decisions”, Strategy Science, Vol. 3 No. 4, pp. 658-667, doi: 10.1287/stsc.2018.0071.

Lovallo, D., Cristofaro, M. and Flyvbjerg, B. (2023), “Governing large projects: a three-stage process to get it right”, Academy of Management Perspectives, Vol. 37 No. 2, pp. 138-156, doi: 10.2139/ssrn.4526068.

Luan, S., Reb, J. and Gigerenzer, G. (2019), “Ecological rationality: fast-and-frugal heuristics for managerial decision making under uncertainty”, Academy of Management Journal, Vol. 62 No. 6, pp. 1735-1759, doi: 10.5465/amj.2018.0172.

Luoma, J. and Martela, F. (2021), “A dual-processing view of three cognitive strategies in strategic decision making: intuition, analytic reasoning, and reframing”, Long Range Planning, Vol. 54 No. 3, 102065, doi: 10.1016/j.lrp.2020.102065.

March, J.G. and Simon, H.A. (1958), Organizations, Wiley, New York.

Massaro, M., Dumay, J. and Guthrie, J. (2016), “On the shoulders of giants: undertaking a structured literature review in accounting”, Accounting, Auditing and Accountability Journal, Vol. 29 No. 5, pp. 767-801, doi: 10.1108/aaaj-01-2015-1939.

*Midavaine, J., Dolfsma, W. and Aalbers, R. (2016), “Board diversity and R&D investment”, Management Decision, Vol. 54 No. 3, pp. 558-569, doi: 10.1108/md-09-2014-0574.

Miller, C.C. and Ireland, R.D. (2005), “Intuition in strategic decision making: friend or foe in the fast-paced 21st century?”, Academy of Management Perspectives, Vol. 19 No. 1, pp. 19-30, doi: 10.5465/ame.2005.15841948.

Miocevic, D. (2021), “Investigating strategic responses of SMEs during COVID-19 pandemic: a cognitive appraisal perspective”, BRQ Business Research Quarterly, Vol. 26 No. 4, pp. 313-326, doi: 10.1177/23409444211005779.

Mueller, R. (1968), “The managementality gap”, Management Decision, Vol. 2 No. 4, pp. 208-213, doi: 10.1108/eb000862.

*Mueller-Saegebrecht, S. (2024), “Business model innovation decisions: the role of group biases and risk willingness”, Management Decision, Vol. 62 No. 13, pp. 69-108.

*Mukherji, A. and Hurtado, P. (2001), “Interpreting, categorizing and responding to the environment: the role of culture in strategic problem definition”, Management Decision, Vol. 39 No. 2, pp. 105-112, doi: 10.1108/eum0000000005416.

Neely, J., Lovelace, J.B., Cowen, A.P. and Hiller, N.J. (2020), “Metacritiques of upper Echelons theory: verdicts and recommendations for future research”, Journal of Management, Vol. 46 No. 6, pp. 1029-1062.

*Norris, J.I., Casa de Calvo, M.P. and Mather, R.D. (2020), “Managing an existential threat: how a global crisis contaminates organizational decision-making”, Management Decision, Vol. 58 No. 10, pp. 2117-2138, doi: 10.1108/md-08-2020-1034.

*Okoli, J. and Watt, J. (2018), “Crisis decision-making: the overlap between intuitive and analytical strategies”, Management Decision, Vol. 56 No. 5, pp. 1122-1134, doi: 10.1108/md-04-2017-0333.

*Olson, B.J., Parayitam, S., Cristofaro, M., Bao, Y. and Yuan, W. (2023), “CEO anger: a catalyst for error recognition and learning”, Management Decision, Vol. 62 No. 13, pp. 1-25, doi: 10.1108/md-12-2022-1750.

O’Shaughnessy, N.J. (1984), “Strategy and U.S. cultural bias”, Management Decision, Vol. 22 No. 3, pp. 22-32.

*Panagiotou, G. (2005), “The impact of managerial cognitions on the structure‐conduct‐performance (SCP) paradigm: a strategic group perspective”, Management Decision, Vol. 44 No. 3, pp. 423-441, doi: 10.1108/00251740610656296.

*Parker, L.D. (1980), “Evaluating group decision‐making in the corporate environment”, Management Decision, Vol. 18 No. 1, pp. 35-44, doi: 10.1108/eb001232.

*Pate, L.E. (1987), “Understanding human behaviour”, Management Decision, Vol. 25 No. 6, pp. 58-64, doi: 10.1108/eb001477.

Picone, P.M., Dagnino, G.B. and Minà, A. (2014), “The origin of failure: a multidisciplinary appraisal of the hubris hypothesis and proposed research agenda”, Academy of Management Perspectives, Vol. 28 No. 4, pp. 447-468, doi: 10.5465/amp.2012.0177.

*Picone, P.M., Galvagno, M. and Pisano, V. (2024), “Hubris research in business: taking stock and moving forward”, Management Decision, Vol. 62 No. 1, pp. 1-24, doi: 10.1108/md-12-2022-1653.

Powell, T.C., Lovallo, D. and Fox, C.R. (2011), “Behavioral strategy”, Strategic Management Journal, Vol. 32 No. 13, pp. 1369-1386, doi: 10.1002/smj.968.

Rabetino, R., Kohtamäki, M. and Federico, J.S. (2021), “A (re) view of the philosophical foundations of strategic management”, International Journal of Management Reviews, Vol. 23 No. 2, pp. 151-190, doi: 10.1111/ijmr.12244.

Raisch, S. and Krakowski, S. (2021), “Artificial intelligence and management: the automation–augmentation paradox”, Academy of Management Review, Vol. 46 No. 1, pp. 192-210, doi: 10.5465/amr.2018.0072.

*Ralston, B. (1985), “Group participative decision making: the management style of the future”, Management Decision, Vol. 23 No. 5, pp. 51-56, doi: 10.1108/eb001391.

*Ramya, S.M. and Baral, R. (2021), “‘Mind’ matters! A conceptual framework using mental models and green nudging to drive corporate environmental responsibility”, Management Decision, Vol. 59 No. 4, pp. 719-731, doi: 10.1108/md-01-2019-0061.

Randolph-Seng, B. (2022), “The will to still manage: over a half-century of Management Decision”, Management Decision, Vol. 60 No. 1, pp. 1-3, doi: 10.1108/md-01-2022-168.

*Rice, G.H. (1980), “But how do managers make decisions?”, Management Decision, Vol. 18 No. 4, pp. 194-202, doi: 10.1108/eb001239.

*Ridge, J.W., Kern, D. and White, M.A. (2014), “The influence of managerial myopia on firm strategy”, Management Decision, Vol. 52 No. 3, pp. 602-623, doi: 10.1108/md-01-2013-0037.

*Selart, M. (2005), “Understanding the role of locus of control in consultative decision‐making: a case study”, Management Decision, Vol. 43 No. 3, pp. 397-412, doi: 10.1108/00251740510589779.

*Shoham, A. and Fiegenbaum, A. (2002), “Competitive determinants of organizational risk‐taking attitude: the role of strategic reference points”, Management Decision, Vol. 40 No. 2, pp. 127-141, doi: 10.1108/00251740210422802.

Shrestha, Y.R., Ben-Menahem, S.M. and Von Krogh, G. (2019), “Organizational decision-making structures in the age of artificial intelligence”, California Management Review, Vol. 61 No. 4, pp. 66-83, doi: 10.1177/0008125619862257.

Simon, H.A. (1947), Administrative Behavior, Free Press, New York City, NY.

*Tabesh, P. and Vera, D.M. (2020), “Top managers' improvisational decision-making in crisis: a paradox perspective”, Management Decision, Vol. 58 No. 10, pp. 2235-2256, doi: 10.1108/md-08-2020-1060.

*Tello, S., Latham, S. and Kijewski, V. (2010), “Individual choice or institutional practice: which guides the technology transfer decision‐making process?”, Management Decision, Vol. 48 No. 8, pp. 1261-1281, doi: 10.1108/00251741011076780.

Thaler, R.H. (1985), “Mental accounting and consumer choice”, Marketing Science, Vol. 4 No. 3, pp. 199-214, doi: 10.1287/mksc.4.3.199.

Thaler, R.H. (2000), “From homo economicus to homo sapiens”, Journal of Economic Perspectives, Vol. 14 No. 1, pp. 133-141, doi: 10.1257/jep.14.1.133.

*Tikkanen, H., Lamberg, J.A., Parvinen, P. and Kallunki, J.P. (2005), “Managerial cognition, action and the business model of the firm”, Management Decision, Vol. 43 No. 6, pp. 789-809, doi: 10.1108/00251740510603565.

Todd, P.M. and Gigerenzer, G.E. (2012), “What is ecological rationality”, in Todd, P.M., Gigerenzer, G.E. and Research Group, T.A.B.C. (Eds), Ecological Rationality: Intelligence in the World, Oxford University Press, Oxford.

Tranfield, D., Denyer, D. and Smart, P. (2003), “Towards a methodology for developing evidence informed management knowledge by means of systematic review”, British Journal of Management, Vol. 14 No. 3, pp. 207-222, doi: 10.1111/1467-8551.00375.

Tversky, A. and Kahneman, D. (1974), “Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases: biases in judgments reveal some heuristics of thinking under uncertainty”, Science, Vol. 185 No. 4157, pp. 1124-1131, doi: 10.1126/science.185.4157.1124.

*Tyzack, J.E.V. (1967), “The role of the board and the director”, Management Decision, Vol. 1 No. 4, pp. 4-8, doi: 10.1108/eb000810.

Urío, S., Redondo, R. and Gavilan, D. (2022), “The intellectual structure of behavioral strategy: a bibliometric study”, Strategic Management, Vol. 27 No. 1, pp. 4-21, doi: 10.5937/straman2110005u.

Virmani, N., Sharma, S., Kumar, A. and Luthra, S. (2023), “Adoption of industry 4.0 evidence in emerging economy: behavioral reasoning theory perspective”, Technological Forecasting and Social Change, Vol. 188 No. 122317.

Waldman, D.A. and Sparr, J.L. (2023), “Rethinking diversity strategies: an application of paradox and positive organization behavior theories”, Academy of Management Perspectives, Vol. 37 No. 2, pp. 174-192.

Wang, Q., Yang, S., Liu, M., Cao, Z. and Ma, Q. (2014), “An eye-tracking study of website complexity from cognitive load perspective”, Decision Support Systems, Vol. 62, pp. 1-10, doi: 10.1016/j.dss.2014.02.007.

White, J. (1984), “Corporate culture and corporate success”, Management Decision, Vol. 22 No. 4, pp. 14-19.

Wren, D.A. and Bedeian, A.G. (2020), The Evolution of Management Thought, John Wiley & Sons, Hoboken, NJ.

*Wright, A. (2005), “The role of scenarios as prospective sensemaking devices”, Management Decision, Vol. 43 No. 1, pp. 86-101, doi: 10.1108/00251740510572506.

Yamini, S. (2021), “Behavioral perspective of newsvendor ordering decisions: review, analysis and insights”, Management Decision, Vol. 59 No. 2, pp. 240-257, doi: 10.1108/md-07-2019-0975.

*Yu, C., Wang, T. and Gu, X. (2022), “Collective reputation cognition, network competence and enterprise innovation performance”, Management Decision, Vol. 60 No. 3, pp. 567-588, doi: 10.1108/md-10-2019-1420.

Yu, X., Liu, T., He, L. and Li, Y. (2023), “Micro-foundations of strategic decision-making in family business organisations: a cognitive neuroscience perspective”, Long Range Planning, Vol. 56 No. 5, 102198, doi: 10.1016/j.lrp.2022.102198.

Further reading

Belton, I.K. and Dhami, M.K. (2020), “Cognitive biases and debiasing in intelligence analysis”, in Routledge Handbook of Bounded Rationality, Routledge, pp. 548-560.

Bijou, S.W., Peterson, R.F. and Ault, M.H. (1968), “A method to integrate descriptive and experimental field studies at the level of data and empirical concepts”, Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, Vol. 1 No. 2, pp. 175-191, doi: 10.1901/jaba.1968.1-175.

Doz, Y.L. (1996), “The evolution of cooperation in strategic alliances: initial conditions or learning processes?”, Strategic Management Journal, Vol. 17 No. S1, pp. 55-83, doi: 10.1002/smj.4250171006.

Guercini, S. and Milanesi, M. (2022), “Foreign market entry decision-making and heuristics: a mapping of the literature and future avenues”, Management Research Review, Vol. 45 No. 9, pp. 1229-1246, doi: 10.1108/mrr-11-2021-0806.

Judge, T.A., Piccolo, R.F. and Kosalka, T. (2009), “The bright and dark sides of leader traits: a review and theoretical extension of the leader trait paradigm”, The Leadership Quarterly, Vol. 20 No. 6, pp. 855-875, doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2009.09.004.

Larrick, R.P. (2004), “Debiasing”, in Koehler, D.J. and Harvey, N. (Eds), Blackwell Handbook of Judgment and Decision Making, Blackwell, Oxford, pp. 316-338.

*Mageean, P.V. (1984), “Changing emphases in the business world – “Multum non Multa”, Management Decision, Vol. 22 No. 4, pp. 20-35, doi: 10.1108/eb001355.

Maljers, F.A. (1990), “Strategic planning and intuition in Unilever”, Long Range Planning, Vol. 23 No. 2, pp. 63-68, doi: 10.1016/0024-6301(90)90200-n.

McGinnis, M.A. (1984), “The key to strategic planning: integrating analysis and intuition”, Sloan Management Review, Vol. 26 No. 1, pp. 45-52.

Mitchell, R.K., Randolph-Seng, B. and Mitchell, J.R. (2011), “Socially situated cognition: imagining new opportunities for entrepreneurship research”, Academy of Management Review, Vol. 36 No. 4, pp. 774-776, doi: 10.5465/amr.2011.65554754.

Neely, B.H. Jr, Lovelace, J.B., Cowen, A.P. and Hiller, N.J. (2020), “Metacritiques of upper echelons theory: verdicts and recommendations for future research”, Journal of Management, Vol. 46 No. 6, pp. 1029-1062, doi: 10.1177/0149206320908640.

Osborne, R.L. (1995), “The essence of entrepreneurial success”, Management Decision, Vol. 33 No. 7, pp. 4-9, doi: 10.1108/00251749510090520.

Powell, T.C. (2011), “Neurostrategy”, Strategic Management Journal, Vol. 32 No. 13, pp. 1484-1499, doi: 10.1002/smj.969.

*Ranganathan, K. (2021), “Do personal values explain variation in satisficing measures of risk?”, Management Decision, Vol. 59 No. 7, pp. 1642-1663, doi: 10.1108/md-08-2019-1115.

Rusetski, A. (2014), “Pricing by intuition: managerial choices with limited information”, Journal of Business Research, Vol. 67 No. 8, pp. 1733-1743, doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2014.02.020.

Corresponding author

Matteo Cristofaro can be contacted at: matteo.cristofaro@uniroma2.it

Related articles