Stigma toward individuals with intellectual disabilities and severe mental disorders: analysis of postgraduate university students’ perceptions

Alvaro Moraleda Ruano (University Camilo Jose Cela, Villanueva de la Cañada, Spain)
Diego Galán-Casado (Department of Educational Theory and Social Pedagogy, UNED, Madrid, Spain)

Advances in Mental Health and Intellectual Disabilities

ISSN: 2044-1282

Article publication date: 27 August 2024

Issue publication date: 5 September 2024

144

Abstract

Purpose

The purpose of this study is to delve into societal stigma surrounding severe mental disorders and intellectual disabilities, emphasizing gender differences and students’ proximity influence.

Design/methodology/approach

Involving 572 Spanish master’s students, this nonexperimental study categorizes participants based on contact frequency, using Goratu and CAEE questionnaires to measure stigma.

Findings

Gender-based stigma differences are absent; however, increased contact correlates with lower stigma levels. Notably, greater closeness is associated with more positive attitudes toward intellectual disability, resulting in diminished stigma toward severe mental disorders.

Practical implications

This research sheds light on the pervasive stigma faced by individuals with intellectual disabilities and severe mental disorders among postgraduate university students. Notably, the recognition of widespread stigma among individuals with higher education highlights a more significant societal problem. The findings underscore the urgent need for targeted interventions, especially in higher education contexts, to enhance understanding and reduce societal bias.

Social implications

By identifying factors influencing stigma and emphasizing the importance of contact in fostering empathy, the study lays the groundwork for informed socioeducational strategies. These strategies have the potential to promote inclusivity, challenge stereotypes and contribute to the well-being and social integration of those affected by intellectual disabilities and severe mental disorders.

Originality/value

The findings highlight the efficacy of direct contact in reducing stigma and underscore the necessity for nuanced understanding. The study suggests fostering positive attitudes through increased contact can combat prejudice and promote social inclusion. Nevertheless, further research is crucial to explore factors influencing stigma reduction and design comprehensive socioeducational interventions addressing diverse cultural proficiencies. This study contributes valuable insights for mitigating stigma, fostering inclusivity and informing future interventions.

Keywords

Citation

Moraleda Ruano, A. and Galán-Casado, D. (2024), "Stigma toward individuals with intellectual disabilities and severe mental disorders: analysis of postgraduate university students’ perceptions", Advances in Mental Health and Intellectual Disabilities, Vol. 18 No. 3, pp. 125-139. https://doi.org/10.1108/AMHID-04-2024-0012

Publisher

:

Emerald Publishing Limited

Copyright © 2024, Emerald Publishing Limited


Clinical impact statement

Our research sheds light on the pervasive stigma faced by individuals with intellectual disabilities and severe mental disorders among postgraduate university students. Notably, the recognition of widespread stigma among individuals with higher education highlights a more significant societal problem. Our findings underscore the urgent need for targeted interventions, especially in higher education contexts, to enhance understanding and reduce societal bias. By identifying factors influencing stigma and emphasizing the importance of contact in fostering empathy, our study lays the groundwork for informed socioeducational strategies. These strategies have the potential to promote inclusivity, challenge stereotypes and contribute to the well-being and social integration of those affected by intellectual disabilities and severe mental disorders.

1. Introduction

Stigma refers to negative reactions, actions or certain beliefs toward a particular group (Corrigan et al., 2009; Corrigan et al., 2010). Stigma manifests through three sociocognitive processes: stereotypes, prejudices and discrimination. Stereotypes refer to the categorization of information about different social groups, prejudices are the result of cognitive and affective responses to stereotypes and discrimination is exhibited through the development of negative behaviors toward individuals or groups due to their group membership (Catalano et al., 2021; Corrigan, 2002, 2004).

The phenomenon of stigma is not a new reality; cultural norms that forbid attributes or traits considered shameful have existed throughout history, with documented evidence of shame and prejudice toward differences dating back to ancient societies (Scambler, 2009).

Regarding the effects of stigma, three specific factors impacting daily life are noteworthy. First, there are profound effects on psychological well-being, wherein negative perceptions pose a threat to one’s self-concept and self-esteem, often erroneously associating the affected group with a propensity for violence or danger. Second, the effects on well-being, stemming from threats to personal identity in stigma, highlight the link between negative perceptions and their impact. Stigmatization may lead to rejection, affecting mental and emotional well-being. Finally, the impact extends to interpersonal relationships, specifically to caregivers and family members (Lowell and Wetherell, 2019), as heightened levels of stress and anxiety arise from reactions received from others (Quiles del Castillo, 2019; Link et al., 1999; Thompson et al., 2002). This generates what is known as courtesy stigma or stigma by association, defined as the stigma experienced by individuals due to their close association with a person who has a stigmatizing characteristic (Abojabel and Werner, 2019; Lowell and Wetherell, 2019; Werner et al., 2020).

These negative effects, hindering proper rehabilitation, can persist beyond the subsiding of psychiatric symptoms or difficulties in leading a normalized life (Link et al., 1987). Reluctance to seek specialized help (Corrigan et al., 2010), limited social interactions, difficulties in performing daily activities or even active job seeking (Kranke et al., 2010; Naslund et al., 2016) are some of the most significant consequences.

In addition, linked to stigma, the phenomenon of self-stigma must be highlighted, intimately related to the person’s perception of themselves, implying the acceptance of negative stereotypes and public attitudes (Corrigan and Shapiro, 2010; Livingston and Boyd, 2010; Lucksted and Drapalski, 2015). Similarly, another type of stigma, known as institutional, is linked to the policies of institutions, both public and private (Mascayano-Tapia et al., 2015).

In the context of social exclusion, individuals with intellectual disability (ID) and those affected by severe mental disorder (SMD) face persistent disparities in health, housing and employment, becoming victim of social stigma and structural stigma. For instance, in the healthcare domain, they have less access to health information, encounter lower quality professional care and face poorer service accessibility due to discriminatory internal policies (Bolster-Foucault et al., 2021; Ditchman et al., 2013; Livingston, 2020; Mason-Whitehead and Mason, 2007; Min and Wong, 2017). As stated by Plena Inclusión (2023), ID is not an illness, although it may sometimes result from one. While ID implies difficulties in intellectual functioning, SMD encompasses mood, personality and behavioral disorders. Despite their distinctions, this study explores both groups, often perceived similarly (Platt et al., 2019). The style guide on mental health and media (Fábregas et al., 2018) emphasizes that mental illness does not imply lower intellectual capacity and can manifest throughout life. The general understanding of these conditions and societal responses could significantly influence their real prognosis (Littlewood, 2006).

For the groups addressed in this research, it is crucial to note that SMD and ID, although distinct, may intersect. This underscores the differences in stigmatization processes and emphasizes the need for greater knowledge of the population so as not to equate both realities. In the case of SMD, there is a higher prevalence of ideas related to danger, violence, the impossibility of recovery, or difficulties in leading an independent life. In contrast, in the case of ID, attitudes are usually more linked to paternalism, condescension, low intelligence or the need for assistance though feelings of pity, the development of abusive behaviors or even the idea that this group should study, work and live in segregated contexts are also present (Cangas and Galván, 2020; Fresán et al., 2010; Moraleda et al., 2023b; Jansen-Van Vuuren and Aldersey, 2020; Moreno et al., 2022; Pace et al., 2010), significantly impacting their quality of life (Ali et al., 2012). However, in both cases, these prejudices and ideas ingrained in social imagination hinder normalization and social participation for these groups.

Studies conducted within university settings have highlighted the efficacy of training programs or interactive interventions in diminishing stigma and enhancing awareness about these groups. For instance, the study conducted by Marino and Bilge (2023) showed enhanced perceptions of individuals with ID through focus groups. Similarly, Harrison et al. (2019) and Jansen-Van Vuuren and Aldersey (2020) showed the significance of active university participation in initiatives aimed at combating prejudices and dispelling misconceptions about ID, emphasizing the pivotal role of direct engagement. In a different approach, Gillespie-Lynch et al. (2015) used online training to enhance understanding of individuals with ID, facilitating the maintenance of meaningful social interactions.

In relation to individuals with SMD, authors such as Gervás et al. (2022) referred to communication campaigns aimed at combating the stigma of psychiatric disorders and encouraging students to consult psychologists and psychiatrists, as well as maintaining contact with individuals with SMD in faculties, improving both knowledge about the group and self-care regarding mental health. Likewise, Moraleda et al. (2023a) and Cerully et al. (2018), through educational documentaries and direct contact, demonstrated how these interventions increased students’ comfort in asking about mental illnesses and temporarily reduced stigmatizing attitudes. On the other hand, initiatives such as the Inclúyete program, where students from the University of Almería engage in sports and cultural activities with individuals with SMD, allow for the reduction of prejudices or the serious game “Stigma Stop” (Cangas et al., 2017a; 2017b), which, virtually, provides students with the opportunity to maintain contact with individuals with mental health issues, allowing for the eradication of misconceptions about severe mental disorders such as schizophrenia.

Therefore, the active fight against stigma toward individuals with SMD and ID requires multiple measures (López et al., 2008), where understanding the attitudes of the population and their assessment, especially in higher education contexts (Wada et al., 2019; Zabaleta González et al., 2023), is one of the fundamental steps to subsequently design and implement socioeducational interventions, as previously mentioned, with the aim of increasing knowledge about the potential of these groups (Babik and Gardner, 2021; Casañas and Lalucat, 2018; Mampaso et al., 2024; Wang et al., 2021). This reality will help reduce social distancing (Blundell et al., 2016; Macmillan et al., 2014; Ouellette‐Kuntz et al., 2010) and counteract the lack of information and ignorance leading to pessimism about the capacities and possibilities of inclusion (Ceballos and Casanovas, 2017).

2. Method

The purpose of this study focuses on two distinct objectives. First, it seeks to identify possible potential differences in stigma toward individuals with ID and SMD concerning gender. This is prompted by numerous studies that have identified variations in these variables (Li et al., 2012; Zabaleta González et al., 2023; Muñoz López, 2022; Vera Noriega et al., 2022) among participants with a higher educational level, specifically postgraduate students. Second, the aim is to analyze potential disparities in this stigma toward people with ID and SMD, considering the proximity of students to individuals with ID and SMD. This proximity is classified into three different levels: no contact, occasional contact and frequent contact.

2.1 Design

This study adopts a cross-sectional research design with an ex post facto nature. This approach enables us to examine the relationships between variables at a single point in time while also exploring the causes and awareness of a phenomenon that cannot occur because it has already happened (Campbell and Stanley, 1963; Fox, 1981; Kerlinger, 1987; Mateo, 1997).

2.2 Sample

A sample of 572 postgraduate students has been used through nonprobabilistic convenience sampling, all belonging to a Spanish university in 2023. The choice of postgraduate students was based on the expectation that this group would represent a higher educational level, which allowed us to explore the impact of stigma even among individuals with a high level of education. Descriptive statistical analyses indicated an average age (M) of 35.12 and a standard deviation (SD) of 8.35, with a range between 22 and 59 years. The sample has been dichotomized by gender, with men accounting for 48.25% (M = 35.12; SD = 8.35; n = 276) and women 51.75% (M = 33.31; SD = 7.36; n = 296).

2.3 Instrument

We inquired with the students about their age, gender, educational background and profession. Additionally, we explored their contact with individuals with ID and SMD, giving them the option to specify the frequency as never, occasionally or frequently. Moreover, two instruments were used:

  1. The Goratu questionnaire “Percepciones sobre las personas con discapacidad intelectual” (Cabezas et al., 2022), with a Cronbach’s α reliability of 0.719 and validated through an exploratory factor analysis applying the principal component method for factor extraction and subsequent factorial analysis to determine the optimal number of factors. This questionnaire, using a Likert scale ranging from 1 to 4 (strongly agree, somewhat agree, disagree and strongly disagree), shows the highest stigma level as the score decreases. It consists of 15 items, distributed across 5 factors, which also reflects an overall score:

    • Factor 1: Ideas related to infantile status, lack of personal competencies (hereafter, infantilization).

    • Factor 2: Ideas related to emotional status, well-being with oneself (hereafter, emotional well-being).

    • Factor 3: Ideas related to social status, interest in participating in inclusive vs segregated spaces (hereafter, inclusion).

    • Factor 4: Ideas related to interpersonal relationships (hereafter, interpersonal relationships).

    • Factor 5: Ideas related to self-determination capacity (hereafter, self-determination).

  2. The “Actitudes de los Estudiantes hacia la Esquizofrenia” questionnaire (CAEE). This is the adaptation into Spanish of the Questionnaire on Students’ Attitudes toward Schizophrenia (Schulze et al., 2003), whose psychometric properties in Spanish samples denote a reliability as internal consistency Cronbach’s α of 0.95 and a bifactorial structure with a total explained variance of 59.94%, like the results of the original instrument (Navarro et al., 2017). This questionnaire, utilizing a Likert scale (with options: I agree, I disagree and I am not sure), shows the highest stigma level as the score increases. Regarding its structure, it consists of 19 items, divided into two subscales, also with a total score:

    • Subscale fear/danger toward people with schizophrenia (hereafter, danger).

    • Subscale stereotypes toward people with schizophrenia (hereafter, stereotypes).

This inclusion is consistent with the study’s emphasis on addressing mental health issues, as schizophrenia is chosen as an illustrative example due to its widespread recognition and prevalence in the collective consciousness of both the scientific community and the general public.

2.4 Procedure

It is noteworthy that data collection, carried out via the online platform Google Forms in 2023, was conducted on the principle of nonintervention, through which complete independence of the population to be analyzed was sought, giving total freedom to respond to the survey and resulting in the voluntary participation of participants, with no financial compensation for being part of the study and respecting the anonymous and confidential nature of the participants. Invitations were extended to 702 postgraduate students from a Spanish university, resulting in an 81.48% participation rate.

Furthermore, this study used a cross-sectional design using an online survey methodology, which allowed for the efficient collection of data from a large sample of postgraduate students. This approach enabled us to explore the attitudes and perceptions of individuals with different educational backgrounds toward individuals with intellectual disabilities and severe mental disorders.

Additionally, it was ensured with the informed consent for data processing solely for research purposes, seeking to respect the ethical principles regarding scientific research, as stated in the Declaration of Helsinki. The Research Ethics Committee of the University Camilo Jose Cela considers the ethical, methodological, and legal aspects of the study correct and appropriate, with code: 06_23_ISESDI.

2.5 Data analysis

Before exploring the data, the normality assumption was confirmed using Kolmogorov-Smirnov (pK-S > 0.05). According to the results, for gender-related data analysis, parametric statistics were used, specifically the t-test with Cohen's d as the effect size estimator. For the analyses on contact with individuals with ID and SMD, the ANOVA test was performed with the estimated effect size eta squared (η2), with post hoc contrasts using t-test with Cohen's d; using version 26.0 of the Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS) statistical analysis software.

3. Results

The analysis focused on exploring stigma toward individuals with ID and SMD within the sample, distinguishing the results by gender. Subsequently, to address the primary research objective, a thorough examination was conducted to explore potential statistically significant differences in stigma toward individuals with ID and SMD based on gender, as presented in Table 1.

The table provides detailed descriptive statistics and the outcomes of hypothesis testing for independent samples, illustrating the dimensions and total scores analyzed for each gender group. Following hypothesis testing, no statistically significant differences were found in any of the dimensions and total scores analyzed based on gender (p > 0.05).

On the other hand, to address the second research objective, descriptive analyses of stigma toward individuals with ID and SMD in the sample, differentiated by the degree of contact with individuals with ID and SMD (G1: never; G2: occasionally; G3: frequently), resulted in the data presented in Table 2. Table 2 provides a comprehensive overview of stigma levels, allowing us to discern patterns based on the participants’ varying degrees of contact with individuals with ID and SMD.

Thus, possible statistically significant differences in stigma toward people with ID and SMD, differentiated by contact with individuals with ID and SMD, were analyzed, as depicted in Table 3. It is noteworthy that only those post hoc contrasts showing statistically significant differences are presented in Table 4.

As indicated by the preceding tests, statistically significant differences were observed in a large considerable portion of the assessed factors, consistently showing positive values in students with greater contact with individuals with ID or SMD, with a small to moderate effect size, following the interpretation of López-Martín and Ardura-Martínez (2022), ranging from small (d > 0.200) to large (d > 0.800).

Regarding differences between groups based on contact with individuals with SMD, students with more contact had lower levels in variables related to stigma toward people with SMD. Specifically, participants with frequent contact statistically outperformed those who had never contact in SMD danger (t = 4.39, p < 0.001, d = 0.651), SMD stereotypes (t = 3.97, p < 0.001, d = 0.589) and SMD total (t = 4.74, p < 0.001, d = 0.724). The same trend occurred with those who had occasional contact in SMD danger (t = 2.52, p < 0.050, d = 0.355), SMD stereotypes (t = 2.49, p < 0.050, d = 0.316) and SMD total (t = 2.86, p < 0.050, d = 0.263). Conversely, no differences were found in variables on the stigma scale toward people with ID based on contact groups with individuals with SMD.

Finally, regarding differences in groups based on proximity to individuals with ID, students with closer relationships had statistically less stigma toward people with ID than those with less interaction. In detail, students with frequent contact showed lower levels of stigma than those with occasional contact in ID infantilization (t = 2.88, p < 0.050, d = 0.354), ID interpersonal relationships (t = 2.64, p < 0.050, d = 0.492) and ID total (t = 2.39, p < 0.050, d = 0.335); and then those with no contact in ID infantilization (t = 3.57, p < 0.001, d = 0.595), ID inclusion (t = 2.57, p < 0.050, d = 0.397), ID self-determination (t = 2.84, p < 0.050, d = 0.315) and ID total (t = 3.26, p < 0.010, d = 0.812). Additionally, it is noteworthy that students with more contact with individuals with ID also had less stigma toward people with SMD than participants with no contact with individuals with ID, specifically in SMD danger (t = 2.78, p < 0.050, d = 0.652), SMD stereotypes (t = 2.64, p < 0.050, d = 0.318) and SMD total (t = 2.77, p < 0.050, d = 0.347).

4. Discussion

Traditionally, scientific literature has highlighted gender differences among postgraduate students regarding stigma toward individuals with ID, suggesting that women tend to display more positive attitudes, less stigmatizing behaviors and greater openness when addressing issues related to ID (Zabaleta González et al., 2023; Galán Casado et al., 2020; Li et al., 2012; Muñoz López, 2022; Vera Noriega et al., 2022; Wilson and Scior, 2014).

However, our results reveal the absence of significant differences between men and women, specifically concerning gender. This finding is perceived as a positive aspect, suggesting recent changes regarding the visibility and awareness of ID, SMD and other realities, as stated in the report prepared by the Confederación Salud Mental España, & Fundación Mutua Madrileña (2023). This situation significantly contributes to the reduction in prejudices and misconceptions (Hernández-Beltrán et al., 2023; Moraleda et al., 2023a).

On the other hand, the results obtained showed that students with greater contact with individuals with SMD and individuals with ID had a lower level of stigma and therefore expressed less social distancing. Many studies focus on direct contact as a tool to reduce prejudices associated with these groups (Cangas et al., 2022; Gaebel et al., 2002; González Sanguino, 2021). For example, from a more classical perspective, Kolodziej and Johnson (1996) established that contact-based interventions developed in mental health-related contexts were effective in promoting attitude changes toward people with SMD. Similarly, Angermeyer and Matschinger (2004), Henderson et al. (2014), Ahuja et al. (2017) or Martínez-Martínez et al. (2019) ruled that students more familiar with SMD were less likely to classify individuals, a weaker desire for social distancing and more positive attitudes toward the rights of this group. Moreover, using schizophrenia as a reference provides a common starting point for discussing mental health issues, given its extensive study and widespread coverage in various media. Familiarity with the term facilitates understanding and dialogue about the importance of addressing mental health problems in society.

Regarding ID, studies like that of Blundell et al. (2016) or Macmillan et al. (2014) establish that the frequency of contact, proximity and relationship are essential to explaining variation in social distancing along with statistically significant association between contact with individuals with ID and more positive attitudes toward this reality. In line with the above, Scior and Werner (2015) emphasized that contact with individuals with ID plays an important role in attitude change and prejudice reduction, promoting full and active inclusive processes.

Despite appreciating various scientific studies showing the importance of contact to reduce stigma both with individuals with SMD and individuals with ID, the results obtained, after contrasting both variables, show that students with closer relationships and contact with individuals with ID exhibit less stigma toward this specific group and toward individuals with SMD. However, this phenomenon does not occur in reverse, as students with more contact with individuals with SMD reduce stigma toward this group but do not show less stigma toward individuals with ID. The cause of this phenomenon may be a lack of knowledge about the reality with which they have no contact. In this sense, Muñoz López et al. (2009) established that, globally, the level of knowledge is very good regarding treatment and causes of mental illness, whereas it is notably lower on issues related to the differentiation of other realities (specifically ID), which can generate a greater number of associated prejudices. Likewise, Wang et al. (2021) or Babik and Gardner (2021) emphasized the importance of having prior knowledge of ID to improve attitudes toward this population, where simply being in contact with other vulnerable groups may not positively influence the reduction of stigma toward other groups such as individuals with ID.

5. Conclusion

The current study highlights a knowledge gap among students with a higher educational level regarding two very specific groups such as people with ID and people with SMD. This underscores that education alone does not necessarily lead to a reduction in prejudicial and erroneous conceptions. Consequently, there is a pressing need for inclusive education accompanied by extensive campaigns and awareness programs to promote equal opportunities and objective knowledge of all groups, regardless of their characteristics. Furthermore, advocating for an increased number of studies is essential to enhance the quality of life for individuals with ID and SMD by addressing existing prejudices. Additionally, emphasis is placed on the importance of individuals with ID and SMD actively participating in decision-making processes and policy creation that directly impacts them (Moraleda et al., 2023a; 2023b).

Considering the persistence of stigma among individuals with higher education, our findings suggest that addressing stigma in this seemingly educated and cultured population poses a more significant societal challenge. Despite their educational background, individuals with higher education still exhibit levels of stigma, raising concerns about the effectiveness of current educational approaches in fostering more positive attitudes toward individuals with ID and SMD. This observation emphasizes the need for tailored interventions specifically targeting individuals in higher education to ensure a comprehensive reduction in societal bias.

5.1 Limitations

The main limitations of the study focus on a restricted sample, specifically, postgraduate students. This group may not be representative of a broader cultural and social stratum, indicating a discreet selection process, limited understanding of the sample in terms of sociodemographic, academic and professional variables. Other limitations are associated with restricted control of extraneous variables, the use of self-perception elements, limited control of the variables used, the absence of a control group type that does not allow us to make experimental designs, and the lack of follow-up of the study participants, which would have led to making inferences of a higher order.

It is worth noting that using schizophrenia as an illustrative example for mental health discussions stems from its widespread recognition, serving as a relatable starting point. While this choice simplifies dialogue and underscores the importance of addressing mental health, it does not undermine the complexities of other mental health conditions, each warranting specific attention and understanding.

Similarly, in future research, it would be interesting to increase the sample size, transition toward data collection based on capacity tests rather than relying solely on self-perception. This shift implies a move toward more objective methods, potentially reducing biases associated with subjective interpretations and providing more precise measurements. Additionally, conducting interventions with pretest-posttest designs with the presence of a control group, would offer insights into the effect of these on stigma toward individuals with ID or with SMD.

Acknowledgments

This contribution linked to the research project ‘Socio-educational intervention against social stigma towards intellectual disability,’ with the acronym ISESDI in its Spanish acronym, was financed with funds from the 9th research call at Camilo José Cela University.

Positionality statement: Fully cognizant that our identities can exert influence on our scientific approach (Roberts et al., 2020), the authors aspire to furnish the reader with detailed information regarding our backgrounds. Concerning gender, during the manuscript’s composition, both authors self-identified as middle-aged males. Regarding race, both authors identify as individuals of Caucasian Spanish descent.

Author contributions statements: “The contributor roles for this research endeavor were meticulously defined and allocated as follows: Álvaro Moraleda (AM) and Diego Galán-Casado (DG-C) jointly conceptualized the project. AM took the lead in data curation, formal analysis, funding acquisition, investigation, methodology, project administration, resources, software, supervision, validation and original draft writing. DG-C and AM collaborated on visualization and shared responsibilities for both original draft writing and review and editing. This collaborative effort reflects the complementary expertise and contributions of AM and DG-C throughout the entire research process”.

Descriptive statistics and hypothesis testing for independent samples of stigma toward individuals with ID and SMD differentiated by gender

Males Females t-test for independent samples
Stigma measures N M SD N M SD T df p d
ID infantilization 276 14.58 2.64 296 15.02 2.59 −2.017 570 0.044 −0.168
ID emotional well-being 276 10.20 1.50 296 10.14 1.43 0.494 570 0.622 0.041
ID inclusion 276 5.38 1.17 296 5.47 1.04 −0.965 570 0.335 −0.080
ID interpersonal relationships 276 10.07 1.28 296 10.20 1.37 −1.111 570 0.267 −0.093
ID self-determination 276 6.93 1.08 296 6.97 1.03 −0.432 570 0.666 −0.036
ID total 276 47.17 4.94 296 47.80 5.10 −1.500 570 0.134 −0.125
SMD danger 276 3.95 2.25 296 3.74 2.31 1.081 570 0.280 0.090
SMD stereotypes 276 3.94 3.27 296 3.64 2.88 1.193 570 0.233 0.099
SMD total 276 7.89 4.90 296 7.38 4.52 1.303 570 0.193 0.109

Source: Table by author

Descriptive statistics of stigma toward ID and SMI differentiated by contact with individuals with ID and SMD

Contact with individuals with ID Contact with individuals with SMD
Stigma measures N M SD N M SD
ID infantilization G1 32 13.44 0.47 72 14.33 2.188
G2 458 14.80 0.11 316 14.72 2.540
G3 82 15.37 0.30 184 15.14 2.865
ID emotional well-being G1 32 10.19 0.17 72 10.25 1.412
G2 458 10.12 0.06 316 10.15 1.504
G3 82 10.41 0.17 184 10.15 1.418
ID inclusion G1 32 5.13 0.16 72 5.47 0.964
G2 458 5.45 0.05 316 5.35 1.098
G3 82 5.64 0.11 184 5.55 1.149
ID interpersonal relationships G1 32 10.19 0.22 72 10.11 1.205
G2 458 10.07 0.06 316 10.09 1.386
G3 82 10.49 0.12 184 10.23 1.273
ID self-determination G1 32 6.50 0.19 72 6.78 1.091
G2 458 6.96 0.04 316 6.91 1.062
G3 82 7.12 0.11 184 7.11 1.018
ID total G1 32 45.40 0.95 72 46.94 4.097
G2 458 47.44 0.22 316 47.22 5.037
G3 82 48.83 0.57 184 48.18 5.291
SMD danger G1 32 4.88 0.43 72 4.75 2.019
G2 458 3.82 0.10 316 3.91 2.374
G3 82 3.56 0.25 184 3.38 2.090
SMD stereotypes G1 32 3.90 0.55 72 4.86 3.585
G2 458 3.63 0.14 316 3.89 3.203
G3 82 3.22 0.27 184 3.18 2.456
SMD total G1 32 8.50 0.84 72 9.61 5.076
G2 458 7.72 0.22 316 7.79 4.929
G3 82 6.78 0.46 184 6.57 3.828

Notes: Degree of contact with individuals with ID and SMD (G1 = never; G2 = occasionally; G3 = frequently)

Source: Table by author

Hypothesis testing for independent samples of stigma toward individuals with ID and SMD differentiated by contact with ID and SMD

Contact with individuals with ID Contact with individuals with SMD
Stigma measures df1 df2 F p η² Decision F p η² Decision
ID infantilization 2 569 6.37 0.002** 0.031 G3 > G1*** ^ G3 > G2* 2.866 0.058 0.009
ID emotional well-being 2 569 1.44 0.239 0.009 0.141 0.869 0.008
ID inclusion 2 569 3.81 0.034* 0.031 G3 > G1* 2.109 0.122 0.003
ID interpersonal relationships 2 569 3.50 0.031* 0.039 G3 > G2* 0.657 0.519 0.001
ID self-determination 2 569 4.04 0.018* 0.012 G3 > G1* 2.743 0.066 0.011
ID total 2 569 5.74 0.003** 0.029 G3 > G1**^ G3 > G2* 2.666 0.070 0.003
SMD danger 2 569 3.97 0.019* 0.024 G3 > G1** 9.915 <0.001*** 0.032 G3 > G1*** ^ G3 > G2*
SMD stereotypes 2 569 3.01 0.041* 0.000 G3 > G1* 8.288 <0.001*** 0.023 G3 > G1*** ^ G3 > G2*
SMD total 2 569 2.96 0.046* 0.007 G3 > G1* 11.682 <0.001*** 0.035 G3 > G1*** ^ G3 > G2*
Notes:

*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < .001. Degree of contact with individuals with ID and SMD (G1 = never; G2 = occasionally; G3 = frequently)

Source: Table by author

Post hoc tests for stigma toward ID and SMI differentiated by contact with individuals with ID and SMD (only significant values)

G1 G2
Stigma measures t df p d t df p d
Contact with individuals with ID
ID infantilization G3 3.57 569 0.001*** 0.595 2.88 569 0.011* 0.354
ID inclusion G3 2.57 569 0.034* 0.397
ID interpersonal relationships G3 2.64 569 0.023* 0.492
ID self-determination G3 2.84 569 0.013* 0.315
ID total G3 3.26 569 0.003** 0.812 2.39 569 0.045* 0.335
SMD danger G3 −2.78 569 0.015* 0.652
SMD stereotypes G3 −2.64 569 0.038* 0.318
SMD total G3 −2.77 569 0.030* 0.347
Contact with individuals with SMD
SMD danger G3 −4.39 569 <0.001*** 0.651 −2.52 569 0.032* 0.355
SMD stereotypes G3 −3.97 569 <0.001*** 0.589 −2.49 569 0.035* 0.316
SMD total G3 −4.74 569 <0.001*** 0.724 −2.86 569 0.012* 0.263
Notes:

*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01; n = 572; Degree of contact with individuals with ID and SMD (G1 = never; G2 = occasionally; G3 = frequently)

Source: Table by author

References

Abojabel, H. and Werner, P. (2019), “Exploring family stigma among caregivers of persons with Alzheimer’s disease: the experiences of Israeli-Arab caregivers”, Dementia, Vol. 18 No. 1, pp. 391-408, doi: 10.1177/1471301216673920.

Ahuja, K.K., Dhillon, M., Juneja, A. and Sharma, B. (2017), “Breaking barriers: an education and contact intervention to reduce mental illness stigma among Indian college students”, Psychosocial Intervention, Vol. 26 No. 2, pp. 103-109, doi: 10.1016/j.psi.2016.11.003.

Ali, A., Hassiotis, A., Strydom, A. and King, M. (2012), “Self stigma in people with intellectual disabilities and courtesy stigma in family carers: a systematic review”, Research in Developmental Disabilities, Vol. 33 No. 6, pp. 2122-2140, doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2012.06.013.

Angermeyer, M.C. and Matschinger, H. (2004), “The stereotype of schizophrenia and its impact on discrimination against people with schizophrenia: results from a representative survey in Germany”, Schizophrenia Bulletin, Vol. 30 No. 4, pp. 1049-1061, available at: www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15954207

Babik, I. and Gardner, E.S. (2021), “Factors affecting the perception of disability: a developmental perspective”, Frontiers in Psychology, Vol. 12, p. 702166, doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.702166.

Blundell, R., Das, R., Potts, H. and Scior, K. (2016), “The association between contact and intellectual disability literacy, causal attributions and stigma”, Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, Vol. 60 No. 3, pp. 218-227, doi: 10.1111/jir.12241.

Bolster-Foucault, C., Ho Mi Fane, B. and Blair, A. (2021), “Structural determinants of stigma across health and social conditions: a rapid review and conceptual framework to guide future research and intervention”, Health Promotion and Chronic Disease Prevention in Canada, Vol. 41 No. 3, pp. 85-115, doi: 10.24095/hpcdp.41.3.03.

Cabezas, D., Gerolín, M., Canto, A. and Vidorreta, I. (2022), “Propiedades psicométricas del cuestionario goratu ‘percepciones sobre las personas con discapacidad intelectual’ en alumnado de educación secundaria obligatoria y bachillerato”, Revista Complutense de Educación, Vol. 33 No. 2, pp. 311-324, doi: 10.5209/rced.74294.

Campbell, D.T. and Stanley, J.C. (1963), “Experimental and Quasi-Experimental designs for research on teaching”, in Gage, N.L. (Ed.), Handbook of Research on Teaching, Rand McNally, pp. 171-246.

Cangas, A.J. and Galván, B. (2020), “Estudio preliminar de la utilidad de un programa de realidad virtual contra el estigma en salud mental”, Revista INFAD de Psicología. International Journal of Developmental and Educational Psychology, Vol. 1 No. 1, pp. 45-52, doi: 10.17060/ijodaep.2020.n1.v1.1760.

Cangas, A.J., Sánchez-Lozano, I., Aguilar-Parra, J.M. and Trigueros, R. (2022), “Combination of a serious game application and direct contact with mental health patients”, International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, Vol. 20 No. 6, pp. 3274-3284, doi: 10.1007/s11469-022-00752-x.

Cangas, A.J., López, A., Orta, A., Gallego, J., Alias, I., Soriano, C. and Piedra, J.A. (2017a), “Inclúyete: Programa Socioeducativo e Inclusivo para Personas con Trastorno Mental Grave y Estudiantes en la Universidad de Almería”, Edupsykhé. Revista de Psicología y Educación, Vol. 16 No. 1, pp. 86-97.

Cangas, A.J., Navarro, N., Parra, J.M.A., Ojeda, J.J., Cangas, D., Piedra, J.A. and Gallego, J. (2017b), “Stigma-stop: a serious game against the stigma toward mental health in educational settings”, Frontiers in Psychology, Vol. 8, p. 1385, doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01385.

Casañas, R. and Lalucat, L.L. (2018), “Promoción de la salud mental, prevención de los trastornos mentales y lucha contra el estigma. Estrategias y recursos Para la alfabetización emocional de los adolescentes”, Revista de Estudios de Juventud, Vol. 121, pp. 117-132.

Catalano, L.T., Brown, C.H., Lucksted, A., Hack, S.M. and Drapalski, A.L. (2021), “Support for the social-cognitive model of internalized stigma in serious mental illness”, Journal of Psychiatric Research, Vol. 137, pp. 41-47, doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2021.02.014.

Ceballos, I. and Casanovas, R. (2017), “La función y el valor del deporte en AVIFES: educació social”, Revista Dìntervenció Socioeducativa, Vol. 65, pp. 124-140.

Cerully, J.L., Acosta, J.D. and Sloan, J. (2018), “Mental health stigma and its effects on treatment-related outcomes: A narrative review”, Military Medicine, Vol. 183 Nos 11/12, pp. e427-e437, doi: 10.1093/milmed/usx219.

Confederación Salud Mental España, & Fundación Mutua Madrileña (2023), “La situación de la salud mental en españa”, Confederación Salud Mental España y Fundación Mutua Madrileña.

Corrigan, P.W. (2002), “Testing social cognitive models of mental illness stigma: the prairie state stigma studies”, Psychiatric Rehabilitation Skills, Vol. 6 No. 2, pp. 232-254, doi: 10.1080/10973430208408434.

Corrigan, P.W. (2004), “How stigma interferes with mental health care”, American Psychologist, Vol. 59 No. 7, pp. 614-625, doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.59.7.614.

Corrigan, P.W. and Shapiro, J.R. (2010), “Measuring the impact of programs that challenge the public stigma of mental illness”, Clinical Psychology Review, Vol. 30 No. 8, pp. 907-922, doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.06.004.

Corrigan, P.W., Larson, J.E. and Rüsch, N. (2009), “Self-stigma and the ‘why try’ effect: impact on life goals and evidence based practices”, World Psychiatry, Vol. 8 No. 2, pp. 75-81, doi: 10.1002/j.2051-5545.2009.tb00218.x.

Corrigan, P.W., Larson, J.E., Kuwabara, S.A. and Sachiko, A. (2010), “Social psychology of the stigma of mental illness: Public and self-stigma models”, in Maddux y, J.E. and Tangney, J.P. (Eds), Social Psychological Reducción Del Estigma En Salud Mental 107 Foundations of Clinical Psychology, Guilford Press, pp. 51-68, available at: https://bit.ly/3dmb9ss

Ditchman, N., Werner, S., Kosyluk, K., Jones, N., Elg, B. and Corrigan, P.W. (2013), “Stigma and intellectual disability: potential application of mental illness research”, Rehabilitation Psychology, Vol. 58 No. 2, pp. 206-216, doi: 10.1037/a0032466.

Fábregas, M., Tafur, A., Guillén, A., Bolaños, L., Méndez, J.L. and Fernández De Sevilla, P. (2018), “Mental health style guide for the media: words do matter”, Spain Mental Health, Confederation, 2018.

Fox, D. (1981), El Proceso de Investigación En Educación, EUNSA.

Fresán, A., Robles, R., de Benito, L., Saracco, R. and Escamilla, R. (2010), “Development and psychometric properties of a brief instrument to measure the stigma of aggressiveness in schizophrenia”, Actas Espanolas de Psiquiatria, Vol. 38 No. 6, pp. 340-344.

Gaebel, W., Baumann, A., Witte, A.M. and Zaeske, H. (2002), “Public attitudes towards people with mental illness in six German cities”, European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, Vol. 252 No. 6, pp. 278-287, doi: 10.1007/s00406-002-0393-2.

Galán Casado, D., Castillo Algarra, J. and García Tardón, B. (2020), “Deporte e inclusión social en personas con trastorno mental grave (TMG)”, Psychology, Society & Education, Vol. 12 No. 1, pp. 71-83, doi: 10.25115/psye.v0i0.2343.

Gervás, R., García-Ullán, L.L., Amor, V., Bullón, A., Vicente Galindo, P. and Roncero, C. (2022), “Effectiveness and types of interventions to reduce mental Illness-Related stigma among medical university students: a literature review (1997-2020)”, Actas Españolas de Psiquiatría, Vol. 50 No. 2, pp. 106-113, available at: https://actaspsiquiatria.es/index.php/actas/article/view/971.

Gillespie-Lynch, K., Brooks, P.J., Someki, F., Obeid, R., Shane-Simpson, C., Kapp, S.K., Daou, N. and Smith, D.S. (2015), “Changing college students’ conceptions of autism: an online training to increase knowledge and decrease stigma”, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Vol. 45 No. 8, pp. 2553-2566, doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2422-9.

González Sanguino, C. (2021), “El estigma en la salud mental: Estigma implícito y explícito”, [Tesis Doctoral, Universidad Complutense de Madrid], available at: https://docta.ucm.es/bitstreams/b51ad525-9542-4ea1-9019-bef06f3af886/download

Harrison, A.J., Bisson, J.B. and Laws, C.B. (2019), “Impact of an inclusive postsecondary education program on implicit and explicit attitudes toward intellectual disability”, Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, Vol. 57 No. 4, pp. 323-336, doi: 10.1352/1934-9556-57.4.323.

Henderson, C., Noblett, J., Parke, H., Clement, S., Caffrey, A., Gale-Grant, O., Schulze, B., Druss, B. and Thornicroft, G. (2014), “Mental health-related stigma in health care and mental health-care settings”, The Lancet Psychiatry, Vol. 1 No. 6, pp. 467-482, doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(14)00023-6.

Hernández-Beltrán, V., González-Coto, V.A., Gámez-Calvo, L., Suárez-Arévalo, E. and Gamonales, J.M. (2023), “Importancia de las actitudes hacia las personas con discapacidad en educación infantil y primaria. Revisión sistemática”, Bordón. Revista de Pedagogía, Vol. 75 No. 1, pp. 83-110, doi: 10.13042/Bordon.2023.95518.

Jansen-Van Vuuren, J. and Aldersey, H.M. (2020), “Acceptance and belonging for people with IDD across cultures”, Current Developmental Disorders Reports, Vol. 7 No. 3, pp. 163-172, doi: 10.1007/s40474-020-00206-w.

Kerlinger, F.N. (1987), “Investigación del comportamiento”, Técnicas y Metodología, Interamericana.

Kolodziej, M.E. and Johnson, B.T. (1996), “Interpersonal contact and acceptance of persons with psychiatric disorders: a research synthesis”, Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, Vol. 64 No. 6, pp. 1387-1396, doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.64.6.1387.

Kranke, D., Floersch, J., Townsend, L. and Munson, M. (2010), “Stigma experience among adolescents taking psychiatric medication”, Children and Youth Services Review, Vol. 32 No. 4, pp. 496-505, doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2009.11.002.

Li, C., Tsoi, E.W.S. and Wang, J.C.K. (2012), “Chinese college students’ attitudes toward people with intellectual disabilities: differences by study major, gender, contact, and knowledge”, International Journal of Developmental Disabilities, Vol. 58 No. 3, pp. 137-144, doi: 10.1179/2047386912Z.00000000014.

Link, B.G., Cullen, F.T., Frank, J. and Wozniak, J.F. (1987), “The social rejection of former mental patients: understanding why labels matter”, American Journal of Sociology, Vol. 92 No. 6, pp. 1461-1500, doi: 10.1086/228672.

Link, B.G., Phelan, J.C., Bresnahan, M., Stueve, A. and Pescosolido, B.A. (1999), “Public misconceptions of mental illness: labels, causes, dangerousness, and social distance”, American Journal of Public Health, Vol. 89 No. 9, pp. 1328-1333, doi: 10.2105/AJPH.89.9.1328.

Littlewood, R. (2006), “Mental health and intellectual disability: culture and diversity”, Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, Vol. 50 No. 8, pp. 555-560, doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2006.00834.x.

Livingston, J.D. (2020), “Structural stigma in health-care contexts for people with mental health and substance use issues: a literature review”, Mental Health Commission of Canada, available at: www.mentalhealthcommission.ca/wp-content/uploads/drupal/2020-07/structural_stigma_in_healthcare_eng.pdf

Livingston, J.D. and Boyd, J.E. (2010), “Correlates and consequences of internalised stigma for people living with mental illness: a systematic review and meta-analysis”, Social Science & Medicine, Vol. 71 No. 12, pp. 2150-2161, doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2010.09.030.

López, M., Laviana, M., Fernández, L., López, A., Rodríguez, A.M. and Aparicio, A. (2008), “La lucha contra el estigma y la discriminación en salud mental: una estrategia compleja basada en la información disponible”, Revista de la Asociación Española de Neuropsiquiatría, Vol. 28 No. 1, pp. 48-83.

López-Martín, E. and Ardura-Martínez, D. (2022), “El tamaño del efecto en la publicación científica”, Educación XX1, Vol. 26 No. 1, pp. 9-17, doi: 10.5944/educxx1.36276.

Lowell, B. and Wetherell, M.A. (2019), “Affiliate stigma, perceived social support and perceived stress in caregivers of children with autism spectrum disorder: a multiple mediation study”, Archives of Psychiatric Nursing, Vol. 33 No. 5, pp. 31-35, doi: 10.1016/j.apnu.2019.08.012.

Lucksted, A. and Drapalski, A.L. (2015), “Self-stigma regarding mental illness: definition, impact, and relationship to societal stigma”, Psychiatric Rehabilitation Journal, Vol. 38 No. 2, pp. 99-102.

Macmillan, M., Tarrant, M., Abraham, C. and Morris, C. (2014), “The association between children’s contact with people with disabilities and their attitudes towards disability: a systematic review”, Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, Vol. 56 No. 6, pp. 529-546, doi: 10.1111/dmcn.12326.

Mampaso, J., Moraleda, Á., Galán-Casado, D. and Ruiz-Vicente, D. (2024), “Stigmatising young people with intellectual disability: perceptions of the main stakeholders at a Spanish university”, Journal of Intellectual & Developmental Disability, Vol. 49 No. 1, pp. 1-11, doi: 10.3109/13668250.2023.2295244.

Marino, M.I. and Bilge, N. (2023), “Silenced and invisible:Students with intellectual disabilities in higher education”, Journal of Postsecondary Education and Disability, Vol. 36 No. 2, pp. 153-165.

Martínez-Martínez, C., Sánchez-Martínez, V., Sales-Orts, R., Dinca, A., Richart-Martínez, M. and Ramos-Pichardo, J.D. (2019), “Effectiveness of direct contact intervention with people with mental illness to reduce stigma in nursing students”, International Journal of Mental Health Nursing, Vol. 28 No. 3, pp. 735-743, doi: 10.1111/inm.12578.

Mascayano-Tapia, F., Lips-Castro, W., Mena-Poblete, C. and Manchego-Soza, C. (2015), “Estigma hacia los trastornos mentales: características e intervenciones”, Salud Mental, Vol. 38 No. 1, pp. 53-58.

Mason-Whitehead, E. and Mason, T. (2007), “Stigma and exclusion in healthcare settings”, in Abrams, D., Christian, J. and Gordon, D. (Eds), Multidisciplinary Handbook of Social Exclusion Research, Wiley, pp. 59-78.

Mateo, J. (1997), La Investigación “Ex-Post-Facto”, UOC.

Min, S.Y. and Wong, Y.L.I. (2017), “Association between community contextual factors and stigma of mental illness in South Korea: a multilevel analysis”, Psychiatric Quarterly, Vol. 88 No. 4, pp. 853-864, doi: 10.1007/s11126-017-9503-1.

Moraleda, Á., Galán-Casado, D. and Cangas, A.J. (2023a), “Las intervenciones socioeducativas en estudiantes de magisterio Para reducir el estigma en la salud mental”, Psicología Educativa, Vol. 29 No. 1, pp. 101-107, doi: 10.5093/psed2023a4.

Moraleda, Á., Galán-Casado, D. and Ruiz-Vicente, D. (2023b), “Abordando el estigma y la inclusión de personas con discapacidad intelectual en españa. Un análisis del marco teórico y normativo”, in Molero, M.M., Simón, M.M., Gázquez, J.J., Molina, P. and Fernández, S. (Comps.), Nuevos Enfoques de Aproximación a la Investigación e Intervención En Contextos Educativos, Dykinson, pp. 125-136.

Moreno, M.I., Morán Suárez, M.L., Gómez Sánchez, L.E., Solís García, P. and Alcedo Rodríguez, M.A. (2022), “Actitudes hacia las personas con discapacidad: una revisión de la literatura”, Revista Española de Discapacidad, Vol. 10 No. 1, pp. 7-27, doi: 10.5569/23405104.10.01.01.

Muñoz López, M. (Dir.) (2022), “El estigma en la población española. Una mirada hacia las personas con problemas de salud mental, sin hogar o con discapacidad intelectual”, UCM-Grupo 5, available at: www.contraelestigma.com/wp-content/uploads/2022/09/Informe-Resultados_-Estigma-psh-salud-mental-y-discapacidad.-Contra-el-Estigma.pdf

Muñoz López, M., Pérez Santos, E., Crespo, M. and Guillen, A.I. (2009), “Estigma y enfermedad mental. Análisis del rechazo social que sufren las personas con enfermedad mental”, Editorial Complutense, available at: https://bit.ly/3bf5eCx

Naslund, J.A., Aschbrenner, K.A., Marsch, L.A. and Bartels, S.J. (2016), “The future of mental health care: peer-to- peer support and social media”, Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences, Vol. 25 No. 2, pp. 113-122, doi: 10.1017/S2045796015001067.

Navarro, N., Cangas, A.J., Aguilar-Parra, J.M., Gallego, J., Moreno-San Pedro, E., Carrasco-Rodríguez, Y. and Fuentes-Méndez, C. (2017), “Propiedades psicométricas de la versión en castellano del cuestionario de las actitudes de los estudiantes hacia la esquizofrenia”, Psychology, Society, & Education, Vol. 9 No. 2, pp. 325-334, doi: 10.25115/psye.v9i2.865.

Ouellette‐Kuntz, H., Burge, P., Brown, H.K. and Arsenault, E. (2010), “Public attitudes towards individuals with intellectual disabilities as measured by the concept of social distance”, Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, Vol. 23 No. 2, pp. 132-142, doi: 10.1111/j.1468-3148.2009.00514.x.

Pace, E., Shin, M. and Rasmussen, S.A. (2010), “Understanding attitudes toward people with down syndrome”, American Journal of Medical Genetics Part A, Vol. 152A No. 9, pp. 2185-2192, doi: 10.1002/ajmg.a.33595.

Platt, J.M., Keyes, K.M., McLaughlin, K.A. and Kaufman, A.S. (2019), “Intellectual disability and mental disorders in a US population representative sample of adolescents”, Psychological Medicine, Vol. 49 No. 6, pp. 952-961, doi: 10.1017/S0033291718001605.

Plena Inclusión (2023), “¿La discapacidad intelectual se cura?”, available at: www.plenainclusion.org/discapacidad-intelectual/recurso/la-discapacidad-intelectual-se-cura/

Quiles del Castillo, M.N. (2019), El Estigma Social. Convivir Con la Mirada Negativa Del Otro, Alianza.

Roberts, S.O., Bareket-Shavit, C., Dollins, F.A., Goldie, P.D. and Mortenson, E. (2020), “Racial inequality in psychological research: trends of the past and recommendations for the future”, Perspectives on Psychological Science, Vol. 15 No. 6, pp. 1295-1309, doi: 10.1177/1745691620927709.

Scambler, G. (2009), “Health-related stigma”, Sociology of Health & Illness, Vol. 31 No. 3, pp. 441-455, doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9566.2009.01161.x.

Schulze, B., Richter-Werling, M., Matschinger, H. and Angermeyer, M.C. (2003), “Crazy? So what! effects of a school project on students' attitudes toward people with schizophrenia”, Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, Vol. 107 No. 2, pp. 142-150, doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0447.2003.02444.x.

Scior, K. and Werner, S. (2015), “Changing attitudes to learning disability: a review of the evidence”, Mencap, University College London & the Hebrew University of Jerusalem.

Thompson, A.H., Stuart, H., Bland, R.C., Arboleda-Florez, J., Warner, R. and Dickson, R.A. (2002), “Attitudes about schizophrenia from the pilot site of the WPA worldwide campaign against the stigma of schizophrenia”, Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, Vol. 37 No. 10, pp. 475-482.

Vera Noriega, J.A., Burruel Valencia, M.A. and Sainz Palafox, M.A. (2022), “Actitudes hacia la discapacidad y su influencia en la percepción de inclusión en estudiantes de educación superior”, Revista de Educación Inclusiva, Vol. 15 No. 2, pp. 45-62.

Wada, M., Suto, M.J., Lee, M., Sanders, D., Sun, C., Le, T.N., Goldman-Hasbun, J. and Chauhan, S. (2019), “University students’ perspectives on mental illness stigma”, Mental Health & Prevention, Vol. 14, p. 200159, doi: 10.1016/j.mph.2019.200159.

Wang, Z., Xu, X., Han, Q., Chen, Y., Jiang, J. and Ni, G.-X. (2021), “Factors associated with public attitudes towards persons with disabilities: a systematic review”, BMC Public Health, Vol. 21 No. 1, p. 1058, doi: 10.1186/s12889-021-11139-3.

Werner, P., Shpigelman, C.N. and Raviv Turgeman, L. (2020), “Family care givers’ and professionals’ stigmatic experiences with persons with early-onset dementia: a qualitative study”, Scandinavian Journal of Caring Sciences, Vol. 34 No. 1, pp. 52-61.

Wilson, M.C. and Scior, K. (2014), “Attitudes towards individuals with disabilities as measured by the implicit association test: a literature review”, Research in Developmental Disabilities, Vol. 35 No. 2, pp. 294-321, doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.11.003.

Zabaleta González, R., Casado Muñoz, R., Martínez Pérez, A. and Lezcano Barbero, F. (2023), “Estigma y actitudes hacia las enfermedades mentales entre los estudiantes universitarios: estudio descriptivo”, Health and Addictions/Salud y Drogas, Vol. 23 No. 1, pp. 333-349, doi: 10.21134/haaj.v23i1.768.

Further reading

Anderson, K. and Austin, J.C. (2012), “Effects of a documentary film on public stigma related to mental illness among genetic counselors”, Journal of Genetic Counseling, Vol. 21 No. 4, pp. 573-581, doi: 10.1007/s10897-011-9414-5.

Corresponding author

Alvaro Moraleda Ruano can be contacted at: amoraleda@ucjc.edu

Related articles